Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T04:59:17.939Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Citizens and Security Threats: Issues, Perceptions and Consequences Beyond the National Frame

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 June 2014

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Citizens are now central to national security strategies, yet governments readily admit that little is known about public opinion on security. This article presents a unique and timely examination of public perceptions of security threats. By focusing on the breadth of security threats that citizens identify, their psychological origins, how they vary from personal to global levels, and the relationships between perceptions of threats and other political attitudes and behaviours, the article makes several new contributions to the literature. These include extending the levels at which threats are perceived from the national versus personal dichotomy to a continuum spanning the individual, family, community, nation and globe, and showing the extent to which perceptions of threat at each level have different causes, as well as different effects on political attitudes and behaviour. These findings are also relevant to policy communities’ understanding of what it means for a public to feel secure.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

The international political landscape in which Britain and other nations operate has been transformed dramatically since the Cold War.Footnote 1 No longer are interests at home and abroad under threat from particular states, but rather from a complex web of security threats said to include international terrorism, weapons of mass destruction, conflict and failed states, pandemics and transnational crime. After 9/11, governments in Britain and America in particular pledged not only to develop a resilient security architecture designed to identify and mitigate against the effects of these threats but, as key objectives, to reassure their publics, heighten collective levels of security among the population and reduce subjective feelings of being ‘threatened’. Importantly, they also sought to achieve these objectives in part by what Jarvis and Lister characterize as ‘conscripting “ordinary” citizens into the state's security apparatuses’,Footnote 2 although there are obvious ambiguities in the effects of these new demands on citizens and the extent to which publics are reassured or made to feel more anxious as a result, as Jarvis and Lister, among others, acknowledge.Footnote 3

The stakes in contemporary threat perceptions are high for governments and citizens alike. While liberal democracies attempt to balance civil liberties and security, a threatened public skews the trade-off toward the latter, tending to favour repression, intolerance, aggressive and exclusionist attitudes toward minorities and targets with different political ideologies, and to show a greater willingness to support war against external sources of threat.Footnote 4 A threatened public may also be more receptive to the enhancement of elite power to enact otherwise unpopular or illiberal policies.Footnote 5 Indeed, the combination of threats and the belief that elites sanction punitive actions that combat threats is particularly dangerous to democracy. Instead of adapting levels of protection to the perceived existence of threats, it may lead to the modulation of threat perceptions in order to justify enhanced levels of protection, such that protection itself may become a threat.Footnote 6 At the extremes, this can result in what Fromm referred to as an escape from freedom,Footnote 7 or what Heymann more recently described as the undermining of nations’ democratic traditionsFootnote 8 – and to the temptation to support charismatic leaders such as Juan Peron and Adolf Hitler.Footnote 9

The events and aftermath of 9/11 have presented what some analysts consider to be a new world order in terms of threat perceptions: it has ‘resulted in chronic changes to schematic representations of the social world as a dangerous and threatening place for many people’,Footnote 10 giving a ‘new urgency to understanding the degree, origins, nature and consequences’ of threats.Footnote 11 The securitization of migration and its increasing association with transnational crime and international terrorism – particularly, though not exclusively, in Europe and North AmericaFootnote 12 – is just one example of ‘“new politics” and “new security” issues’ since 9/11.Footnote 13

At a time when attempts have been made to ‘conscript’ ordinary members of the public into the state's security apparatuses, and national security strategies make a virtue of involving citizens in the risk management cycle, it is more important than ever to understand public perceptions of security threats and their role in contemporary political life. Yet our knowledge of public perceptions of security threats tends to be confined to discrete policy areas such as terrorism or relates to specific areas of personality, predispositions or attitudes such as authoritarianism and tolerance.Footnote 14 The persistence of an elite focus in security studies means that we still know relatively little about the range of issues that individuals regard as security threats, as well as their causes and the levels at which such threats are perceived, for example, as global or national threats.

This article alone cannot address all the issues associated with public perceptions of threat; we do not examine elites’ behaviour, for example. Nor do we argue that contemporary perceptions of threat have reached the extremes described by authors like Fromm. Yet the article does begin to address the urgent questions of what individuals feel threatened by, why this might be the case and what the effects of threat perceptions are for society at large. With this enhanced understanding and deepened analysis of threat perceptions, we can obtain a stronger sense of the relationships between threats and individual and contextual variables, and between threats and political attitudes and behaviour. Such understanding highlights the limitations of the national frame when thinking about public perceptions of security threats. It offers a more nuanced picture of how a citizen's sense of (in)security is linked to the levels at which they perceive certain issues as threatening. In this way, the article contributes to academic knowledge and challenges policy makers to embrace a more variegated approach to the question of ‘national’ security and citizens’ roles within it.

Threat Perceptions and Their Consequences

There has been a great deal of research on different aspects of threat perceptions. This section starts by examining four elements in order to justify and situate our research: the origins of threat perceptions, the focus on specific threats such as terrorism, the distinction between personal and sociotropic threats, and the effects of threat perceptions. We end by presenting our own theory and discussion of the origins and consequences of threats from our analysis of these four elements.

Perceptions of Security Threats and their Origins

For international relations/security studies scholars, policy and academic discourses about threat have broadened and deepened since the end of the Cold War.Footnote 15 Instead of relating exclusively to the survival of the state under anarchical conditions,Footnote 16 understandings of the concept of security threat have broadened beyond the military context to include other sectors such as environmental (threats to earth as biosphere), societal (threats to notions of community), economic (threats to citizens’ welfare) and political (threats to particular national identities).Footnote 17 Such understandings have also deepened to include not only the state as threatened, but also the individual (human security)Footnote 18 and even the planet (eco-security).Footnote 19 Moreover, in the critical constructivist account, security threats are not treated as an objective set of conditions that exists independently of our knowledge, representations and perceptions of them, but rather as a highly politicized category of interpretation.Footnote 20 As typified by the work of the Copenhagen School, perceived threats to security are brought into being performatively via acts of securitization in which a particular issue is framed in terms of an existential threat in, for example, political speeches and media representations, and is accepted as such by legitimizing audiences.Footnote 21

Political psychologists were also heavily influenced by the Cold War and its precursors when thinking about threat. For example, they sought to understand mass and elite behaviour in dictatorships, before turning to explanations of political attitudes such as ethnocentrism and intolerance.Footnote 22 A common theme that continues in contemporary research in these areas is the centrality of threat perceptions. Thus Marcus et al. and Gibson and Gouws argue that normative threat from disliked groups renders individuals both less tolerant and more responsive to information about threats.Footnote 23 Altemeyer, Stenner, and Hetherington and Weiler,Footnote 24 while disagreeing about the relationship between threat and authoritarianism, also all have perceptions of threat at the core of their theories; for example, authoritarians ‘stand about ten steps closer to the panic button than the rest of the population’.Footnote 25 Other research examines threat in particular contexts, such as the threats from immigration,Footnote 26 environmental degradation,Footnote 27 cybercrime,Footnote 28 religious threat,Footnote 29 racism, sexism and stereotype threat,Footnote 30 while much of the context and focus since 9/11 has been on threats associated with international terrorism.

There is, however, uncertainty about the causes of individual perceptions of threats. Some research suggests that the causes are highly unsystematic. Gibson and Gouws refer to ‘the failure of earlier research to account for any variability in threat perceptions’.Footnote 31 Marcus et al. find threat from groups to be ‘exogenous to measures of social background, personality, ideology, and support for the general norms of democracy’.Footnote 32 Similarly, Feldman, Feldman and Stenner, Hetherington and Suhay, and Stenner uncover weak correlations between their measures of threat and authoritarianism.Footnote 33 Yet as the quote from Gibson and Gouws suggests, other research has identified systematic causes of threats. Lavine et al. ‘view a chronic fear of a dangerous and threatening world to be a key component of the authoritarian personality’.Footnote 34 Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior find that perceptions of threats from immigration are linked to cultural identity,Footnote 35 while Huddy et al. show that demographic factors such as education, gender and race, and the predisposition of authoritarianism, are linked to perceptions of threat.Footnote 36 The heightened sense of threat induced by ‘mortality salience’ – both a greater awareness of one's own mortality and feelings of vulnerability – also increases ‘the positivity of evaluations of people and ideas that support one's cultural worldview and the negativity of evaluations of people and ideas that threaten it’.Footnote 37 Finally, Ridout, Grosse and Appleton indicate that media exposure heightens perceptions of ‘modern’ and ‘postmodern’ threats.Footnote 38

In sum, while there is disagreement, variables including dispositions such as authoritarianism, demographic characteristics such as education and age, and exposure to information via media appear to influence threat perceptions. But do they lead to perceptions of greater collective threat, personal threat or both? And if so, are those perceptions of a specific threat or a range of threats?

Perceptions of the Breadth of Threats vs. Specific Threats

The common emphasis in the research on perceptions of threats is on specific threats, such as terrorism or immigration and their intensity, rather than on perceptions of the breadth of threats. This raises two problems that relate to the focus on an ‘extreme stimulus’. First, just as Lupia and Menning argue that ‘researchers tend to ask about people and events that they suspect have caused emotional reactionsFootnote 39 and that the generalizability of results is therefore questionable, the same is true of perceptions of threats, hence the predominant focus in the extant literature on terrorist threats since 2001. Secondly, there is a parallel to the debate in the tolerance literature about measurement error and the conceptual limitations of focusing on a single ‘least-liked group’. The argument there is that it misses the extent to which individuals vary in the breadth of their intolerance. Some individuals may harbour an intense dislike for one group, and others for multiple groups; but concentrating on a single disliked group lumps them together. The parallel with threats is the tendency to examine the intensity of single ‘threats of the moment’ and thus to miss potentially important and consequential differences between individuals for whom the threat of the moment is one of many, and those for whom it is the only threat.Footnote 40 In the tolerance literature, those who have argued for a focus on breadth have claimed that concentrating on an extreme stimulus limits understanding and can lead to erroneous inferences such as of the effects of education.Footnote 41

Sociotropic vs. Personal Threat

A third issue is what we term here to be the level at which threats are perceived. Whether referring to a normative, economic or racial threat, research has largely conceived of threats as either sociotropic- or aggregate-level concerns or personal, individual-level concerns. Most of the research has also found sociotropic threat to be a stronger influence than personal threat, with regard to the economy, race and – more recently – terrorism.Footnote 42 For example, Huddy et al. argue that national threat has far more influence over perceptions of the economic consequences of terrorism, while personal threat is more likely to alter behaviour designed to mitigate threat, such as changing air travel habits to avoid the possibility of harm from a terrorist attack.Footnote 43 They conclude that, as in other policy areas, the influence of the personal on political judgements is limited.

These differences in the effects of sociotropic versus personal judgements on political attitudes and behaviour have attributions as their core explanation. Although sociotropic factors are frequently described as more remote and less vivid than personal concerns,Footnote 44 individuals are said to be more able to make connections between societal conditions and government than they are between their own circumstances and the actions of public officials. It may also be the case that individuals simply view sociotropic factors as more reliable indicators of the likely personal impact of government policies than personal factors,Footnote 45 and, relatedly, because sociotropic factors present the greater threat.Footnote 46 Sociotropic and personal situations may also bring different values and considerations to the fore: sociotropic threat may call to mind a value like freedom of expression, whereas personal threat renders the value of safety more salient.Footnote 47 Another argument is based on information: there is more media coverage of national than local circumstances, more contextualizing of national conditions, and media coverage tends to emphasize sociotropic rather than personal concerns.Footnote 48

However, other research suggests that the national-personal distinction is too limited in two respects. The first limitation is in effects. For example, some studies of economic effects claim that globalization has dampened the influence of national factors while making international economic indicators more salient to individuals;Footnote 49 that is, ‘the global’ is of growing importance relative to the national. Moreover, while the consensus is that personal considerations tend to carry less weight, there is plenty of evidence that both personal and sub-national considerations can matter. Chong, Citrin and Conley argue, for example, that personal considerations exert influence on reasoning when an individual's ‘stakes in the policy are clear’.Footnote 50 Moreover, Huddy et al. note that perspectives on the influence of personal threat could be limited by dependent variables that are frequently related to national consequences.Footnote 51 Jones et al. demonstrate the importance of local context, and Johnston et al. of local unemployment in particular, to voting behaviour in Britain.Footnote 52 Similarly, studies in the United States have shown sub-national influences on economic perceptions,Footnote 53 support for social welfare spending,Footnote 54 immigration,Footnote 55 opinion on the Vietnam WarFootnote 56 and voting behaviour.Footnote 57 Even research that shows a more pervasive influence of sociotropic than of personal considerations often finds that sub-national considerations matter too – but, according to Lau and Heldman, only ‘sporadically, here and there and under particular circumstances’.Footnote 58

The second limitation of the national-personal distinction is in measurement. First, the vast majority of research on sociotropic and personal threats presents them as a dichotomy in which the sociotropic perspective is a national one and personal threat pertains to the individual.Footnote 59 Yet there is no logical reason why sociotropic threat should be confined to the nation. Secondly, measurement of the national-personal dichotomy provides little theoretical or empirical guidance as to whether perceptions of national (let alone global) threats have the same kinds of determinants as personal threats, because the assumption that these are the salient levels of consideration leads to a focus on their effects rather than their causes.

Yet Chanley's research suggests that for an issue such as global warming, conceiving of it as a concern of the planet rather than as a threat that is confined to individual nations may connote a different political outlook and the salience of different value considerations.Footnote 60 Lee, Dallaire and Lemyre's research in Canada also provides a suggestive example in which one of their focus group participants says of terrorism: ‘We're in Canada. It could happen, there, but it worries me in a global sense, the repercussions, international politics with the USA and other countries and the lifestyle change that it brings to us in North America.’Footnote 61 This respondent thinks of terrorism in global terms, but another might see the world primarily in terms of national boundaries or the physical threat terrorism presents to them as an individual.Footnote 62 Indeed, Huddy et al.'s research suggests that authoritarianism may be one influence on whether terrorism is regarded as a global or national threat.Footnote 63 They find that authoritarianism is positively related to preferences for military action and limiting civil liberties in the wake of 9/11 but negatively related to the desire for the United States to be active in world affairs, and unrelated to the desire to see the United States take the leading role in solving international problems. This implies a view of the world that – far from simply being ten steps closer to the panic button – wants action to resolve national disorder and is unsympathetic to action designed to mitigate international disorder.Footnote 64 Kahan's research on cultural world views also points to systematic differences between individuals on the meaning of various issues and therefore on societal dangers.Footnote 65 Such differences may be reflected in tendencies to see threats at one level, for example global rather than personal.

Consequences of Threats

Previous research has explored disparate consequences of threats, including on political attitudes and behaviours,Footnote 66 and effects on other attitudes such as parenting,Footnote 67 the efficacy of government responses,Footnote 68 and the justification of illiberal policies and actions.Footnote 69 In keeping with past research, post-9/11 security threats related to international terrorism have been shown to render individuals more illiberal and less tolerant of difference: making individuals more willing to trade civil liberties for security measures; elevating antipathy toward the entry of immigrants; elevating antipathy toward Muslims as a cultural and religious minority; altering the social identities of majority and minority populations and making them more salient; and leading to a tendency to prefer leaders with particular traits such as strength and charisma.Footnote 70 In addition, a heightened sense of security threat is associated with mortality salience, aggression, and a greater tendency to stereotype outgroups and to dislike stereotype-inconsistent members of outgroups.Footnote 71

Theorizing about the Origins and Consequences of Threats

Previous research has therefore provided mixed perspectives on the origins and consequences of threat perceptions. While we have argued that it is limited by its focus on specific threats and by virtue of the dominant national-personal dichotomy in measurement, certain antecedents (such as mortality salience, authoritarianism and media habits) recur, along with consequences such as hostility toward minority outgroups. We turn now to a discussion of those antecedents and consequences from the broader perspective we have provided.

Mortality salience

Previous research tells us that individuals for whom thoughts of mortality are most accessible are prone to manifest feelings of threat and danger by defending their cultural world views against perceived challenges from outgroups, although this research is usually based on manipulation of mortality salience rather than directly assessing the influence on threat itself.Footnote 72 Our interest is precisely in the relationship between dispositional (that is, unmanipulated) mortality salience and breadth of threats. Given that mortality salience is influential under conditions of mortal physical danger or threats to cultural world view – and is not aroused by mere insecurity, uncertainty or anxiety-producing eventsFootnote 73 – we would expect that mortality salience enhances perceptions of the breadth of global and national threats but not perceptions of sub-national threats if they are more strongly related to issues such as economic insecurity and the anxiety produced by crime.

Authoritarianism

Claims such as Altemeyer's about authoritarian sensitivity to threat would lead us both to expect authoritarianism to affect perceptions of the breadth of threats and for that influence to be consistent across levels.Footnote 74 However, other research on authoritarianism argues either that it is not a cause of elevated threats but is activated for those higher in authoritarianism when the social order is threatened,Footnote 75 or that it is a cause of perceptions of threat, but only in relatively benign societal situations.Footnote 76 We are agnostic on the issue of authoritarian sensitivity to threat, but clearly the latter theories suggest that we may not find a relationship between authoritarianism and perceptions of the breadth of threats.

Media habits

News media may exacerbate perceptions of threatsFootnote 77 through such tendencies as focusing on conflict and catastrophe – on raised but not lowered threat levels, for exampleFootnote 78 – and a tendency to limit coverage of minorities to stories about crime.Footnote 79

Education

Education has been associated with perceptions of lower threat levels; highly educated individuals are thought to have a greater grasp of probabilistic information,Footnote 80 for example the likelihood of a terrorist attack being low, or that immigration, far from being costly to the economy, may be necessary for future economic prosperity given low indigenous birth rates.

Race, religion, age and sex

Sex (being a woman) and religiosity may also heighten threat perceptions, while age may have a negative effect on perceptions of threat.Footnote 81 Finally, British Muslims ‘suffered disproportionately greater levels of stress than respondents from other faiths’ following the 7/7 bombings in London.Footnote 82 Thus we might expect different perceptions of the breadth of global threats from British Muslims because of a different perspective on issues such as religious extremism. Adopting a similar logic about possible differences due to ethnicity, we might also expect an influence of white ethnicity.

Consequences

The range of effects of specific threats that previous research has examined is broad, as outlined above. It indicates that global- and national-level threats, rather than community- or personal-level threats, are particularly salient considerations. However, theorizing about when we should see sub-national influences, rather than simply describing them as sporadic, is important. If international and national government responsibility and global and national implications are the drivers, global- and national-level threats should exert more influence than community or personal threats, for example, on individuals’ willingness to pay increased taxes for enhanced security measures or on vote intention. Similar logic would suggest more evidence of the influence of community and personal threats on attitudes toward outgroups (such as perceptions of immigrants), which are not dependent on attributions of governmental responsibility, may not be seen to have national consequences, and where personal and community threats may be more vivid and emotionally arousing than global or national threats.Footnote 83 In addition, if individuals who are more threatened are more inclined to defend their cultural world views and identity, it is likely that ethnic and racial considerations are central components of that defence via the desire to reduce the relative power of outgroups.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND DATA

Thus our research provides new insights by focusing on: (1) the breadth of security threats that members of the public identify – how many, and whether seeing one type of threat is associated with seeing others; (2) the extent to which perceptions of the breadth of threats vary as individuals move from global to personal security threats; (3) the individual-level influences on perceptions of the breadth of threats; and (4) the relationships between perceptions of threats and other political attitudes and behaviours, such as attitudes toward minority groups and intention to vote in an election.

To gauge the breadth of public perceptions of security threats, we conducted a twenty-five-minute internet survey of 2,004 respondents in Britain from 6–15 June 2012. It included a booster sample of 251 Muslims, a unique group that is both threatened and often seen as threatening by other groups in British society.Footnote 84 Online Appendix Table A1 shows that, other than ethnicity (due to the booster sample), survey respondents were representative of the British population on dimensions of sex, age, region and the party for which they voted in the 2010 general election.Footnote 85

We asked about perceptions of a range of twenty-two potential threats as global, national, community and personal security threats. Some were drawn directly from the tier one, two and three threats identified in the UK government's 2010 National Security Strategy, including terrorism, ‘hostile attacks upon UK cyber space’ and ‘disruption to oil or gas supplies to the UK’, while others emerged as recurrent security threats identified by participants in focus groups we conducted prior to developing the survey, including the economy, immigration, the Far Right, burglary and online fraud.Footnote 86 The survey questions did not define ‘security threat’ for respondents, nor did the list of twenty-two threats privilege any particular type or level of threat; rather, the survey covered potential threats that were domestic and international, economic, political, technological, health related and group based. Indeed, while we would expect identification of some of these threats to be confined to the national or international level, such as the increasing power of Russia and China, others in the list are likely to be seen only as sub-national threats, including knife crime and burglary, while still others could span multiple levels, including the economic crisis, immigration, online fraud and religious extremismFootnote 87 (see the online appendix for details).

Our examination of threats runs from the broadest collective (the global) through national to community-level threats, which are still collective but at a much more intimate and personal level than the state or the globe. Thus we see community-level threats as closer to what is usually meant by personal rather than sociotropic threats.Footnote 88 We operationalize perceptions of threat as the number of issues that people see as global, national, community or personal threats. We did not ask about the intensity with which these threats were felt, or about their content (for example, whether threat from the Far Right stems from it being viewed as dangerous or powerful).Footnote 89 Threats to social cohesionFootnote 90 or from particular groups will be captured by our measure if they rise to the level of security threats. Our measurement of the key antecedents of threat perceptions was as follows:

Mortality salience

Mortality salience was measured using three agree-disagree questions about thoughts of death (details are in the online appendix).Footnote 91

Authoritarianism

We assess authoritarianism through the child-rearing values questions used in the American National Election Study that have become standard; they do not conflate authoritarianism with conservatism or prejudice.Footnote 92

Media habits

We gauge news media effects by examining the influence of the amount of time that respondents claimed to watch news and current affairs programmes on television as a proportion of their total time spent watching television.

Education

We operationalize education with a dummy variable for respondents with a higher education degree or above. We expect that where level of education has an impact, it will be on the identification of threats at the global or national level rather than at lower levels, where perceptions of threats are likely to be less dependent on probabilistic knowledge than on day-to-day experiences in the neighbourhood in which a person lives.Footnote 93

Consequences

We examine views of immigration and of what should be necessary for British citizenship; the extent to which respondents would be willing to pay more in taxation for provision of different kinds of security; and respondents’ certainty that they would vote in the next general election. Attitudes toward immigrants were measured in two ways. First, we asked about the importance of criteria for UK citizenship, ranging from education and work skills to race and religion. Exploratory factor analysis revealed two strong factors, one that we label skills – the need for work skills, education and the ability to speak English – and another that we call culture, which has parallels with Reijerse et al.'s notion of cultural citizenship, which includes being Christian and white (although it also includes being wealthy).Footnote 94 We combined these indicators into separate indexes of the importance of skills and culture for citizenship.

Second, we factor analysed answers to a series of agree-disagree statements about immigration and immigrants, again finding two strong factors: one focused on the effects of immigrants on the economy (and their possible impact on terrorism), and the other focused on culture and multiculturalism. We combined the statements with the highest loadings on these factors into indicators of concerns about the impact of immigration on the economy and terrorism, and about the impact of immigration on Britain's culture. To gauge certainty of voting at the next general election, we constructed a three-point scale based on whether a respondent expected to vote at the next election and whether they could name the party they would vote for (see the online appendix for details). Figure 1 provides an illustrated summary of the model.

Fig. 1 The structural equation models Note: Figure 1 presents the basic approach to modelling without showing arrows for each individual relationship estimated. In the models, Education is specified as an influence on global and national threat only, and White on community and personal threat only.

ANALYSIS

Breadth of Threats

What do perceptions of the breadth of threats at different levels look like? Previous research provides us with little guidance. One possibility is that individuals compartmentalize, identifying a single security threat at different levels, such as terrorism as the principal global threat, the economy as the national threat, knife crime as the community threat and so on. This would lead us to expect limited variation in the breadth of threats at different levels – basically one threat at each level – but variation in what those threats are. A second possibility is that individuals identify more threats that are closer to home; that is, they are more sensitive to threats on their doorstep, which would lead to the expectation of perceptions of more threats at the personal than at the global or national levels.

Figure 2 demonstrates that neither of these possibilities turns out to be accurate. It shows the proportion of respondents identifying particular threats as the number of security threats identified increases from one to seven. The four graphs display the five main security threats identified at each level. The sub-headings for each graph indicate that the average number of perceived threats was greatest at the global level, with fewer issues identified as national-level security threats. The lowest number of perceived threats was personal threats, where perceptions were of roughly two on average.Footnote 95

Fig. 2 Major threats at the global, national, community and individual levels

Figure 2 also demonstrates a pattern in perceptions of threats: the particular threats identified at each level tend not to change as the breadth of security threats increases; that is, it is not the case that a threat like terrorism is particularly important to respondents who identify two threats but not to respondents who identify seven threats. With global threats, for example, terrorism was the most frequently identified threat for respondents who identified one threat, at 30 per cent; racial or religious hate crime ranked fourth at 6 per cent. For respondents identifying seven global threats, terrorism was still the most frequently mentioned, and racial or religious hate crime was the fifth most frequently mentioned. Thus Figure 2 indicates agreement on what the most salient threats are at each level: the differences between respondents perceiving more or fewer threats are not in the issues that are most threatening, but in whether or not an issue crosses a threshold to become a tangible security threat.Footnote 96

Figure 2 also shows that there is variation in the kinds of security threats that are salient at different levels. At the global and national levels the concerns are terrorism, the economy and religious extremism, but whereas immigration and border control are key national security threats, issues like nuclear weapons are seen as more pressing global security threats. Perceptions of salient community- and personal-level security threats are different from global and national threats: the economy and immigration are also threats at the community and personal levels, but burglary, knife crime and online fraud loom much larger.

Huddy et al. characterize the effects of national and personal threats from terrorism as distinct but related.Footnote 97 This notion of threats at different levels as distinct but related appears to be true more broadly: the correlations in the breadth of threats at different levels are mostly in the 0.4–0.5 range (see online Appendix Table A2 for details), which demonstrates that while the specific threats may vary, individuals who see more threats at one level are also likely to see more threats at other levels – they are related. But they are distinct in that the breadth of threats perceived at one level is far less than entirely predictive of the breadth of threats perceived at another level, accounting for no more than one-quarter of the variance.Footnote 98

We now turn to a more detailed examination of the influences on perceptions of the breadth of threats at different levels before looking at the breadth of threats as predictors of: attitudes toward immigrants, policy areas for which respondents would be willing to pay more tax and voting behaviour.

Influences On the Breadth of Threats

Rather than separate models for each level of threat, we examine the four levels of threat simultaneously in a structural equation model.Footnote 99 The predictors for each threat are those depicted in Figure 1. Table 1 presents the estimates. The endogenous variables – perceptions of the breadth of threats – are a count of the number of perceived threats at each level. All of the independent variables, including age, are coded from zero to one, meaning that effect sizes are easily compared (see online appendix for details). We display the estimates in separate columns for each level of threat. We begin by discussing the influences that are common across levels of threat and then turn to those whose effects on perceptions are distinct across levels of threat.

Table 1 Influences on Global, National, Community and Personal/ Family Threats

Note: Estimates are from a generalized structural equation model using Stata 13, and assume the endogenous variables – breadth of threats – have Poisson distributions. Statistical significance may not always appear accurate due to rounding. *p < 0.05, #p < 0.10 (two-tailed test).

The results in Table 1 confirm that there are common influences on perceptions of the breadth of threats that are associated with some individuals systematically perceiving more threats than others. Indeed, several of the variables have pervasive effects on perceptions of the breadth of threats. One is mortality salience. Somewhat contrary to our expectation that the influence of mortality salience would be confined to global and national-level threats, individuals whose mortality was more accessible perceived more threats at all levels.Footnote 100 This suggests that the predisposition to be more aware of one's mortality not only affects perceptions of physical danger but also elevates perceptions of threats that induce uncertainty or anxiety (contrary to some previous researchFootnote 101 ) perhaps because mortality salience leads to a conservative shiftFootnote 102 that elevates perceptions of threat in general, regardless of whether they are physical or uncertainty/anxiety inducing.Footnote 103

Figure 3 shows the maximum effects of mortality salience (and three other variables discussed below) when all other variables are set at their mean or mode. It indicates that, as expected, mortality salience has relatively large effects on perceptions of global and national threats, adding more than one additional threat at each level. But Figure 3 also illustrates the impact of mortality salience on perceptions of personal threats, where although the effect is to raise the number of perceived threats by less than half the amount it does for global threats, it should also be borne in mind that the average number of global threats is more than three times larger than that of personal threats.

Fig. 3 Maximum effects of main influences on breadth of threats Note: Simulations are with all other variables set at their mean or mode (a white, non-Muslim woman, without higher education).

Table 1 shows that watching proportionally more television that covers politics and current affairs also has a consistent influence on harbouring more threats at all levels, echoing Ridout, Grosse and Appleton's finding,Footnote 104 and suggesting either that the world presented on television news heightens the threat perceptions of its most dedicated viewers or that threatened individuals monitor current affairs more closely. The effects of watching political and current affairs television are most consequential at the community and personal levels: the maximum effect of watching politics and current affairs programmes is to raise the breadth of personal threats by about 0.7, while the influence on global and national threats is marginally larger but less noteworthy, given the higher number of threats identified on average at these levels. Age also has a consistent impact on perceptions of the breadth of threats; older individuals feel more threatened, and the effect of age is larger than any of the other variables on breadth of personal threats.

The influence of variables like mortality salience and age does not fluctuate much in terms of their statistical significance on perceptions of the breadth of threats at different levels. Authoritarianism is different, however: while it has the expected positive relationship with perceptions of the breadth of national, community and personal threats – in this sense, authoritarians are chronically threatened – its influence does not extend to perceptions of global-level threats. Thus while authoritarians may be ‘relentlessly sociotropic boundary maintainers’,Footnote 105 their fears about non-conformity or the undermining of approved authority figures do not extend beyond an individual's national borders according to this analysis. Figure 3 shows that the impact of authoritarianism on perceptions of personal threats is also relatively modest.

In addition, we see systematic variation in the effects of the other variables at different levels. For example, sex affects perceptions of threats at all levels other than community (women perceive more threats), while religiosity primarily affects perceptions of local and personal threats. We cannot establish why religiosity influences perceptions of threats at these levels rather than at the global or national levels, but it is noteworthy that attending a place of worship more often is associated with perceptions of more community threats; perhaps places of worship are seen as refuge from the world that is on your doorstep rather than further afield. Education is associated with identifying more global threats but has no influence on perceptions of national threats, while white individuals identify fewer community and personal threats. Lastly, being a British Muslim does not result in perceptions of more threats at the community or personal levels, but Muslims see fewer global and national threats than others.Footnote 106

Effects of Breadth of Threats on Political Attitudes and Behaviour

Our approach is straightforward: in separate models in Table 2, we add additional paths to the structural equation models from threats at each level to variables that capture attitudes toward immigrants, services for which threatened individuals are willing to be taxed more and intention to vote in the next general election. We also add direct paths from each of the exogenous variables in Table 1 to the dependent variables. In other words, we allow variables such as mortality salience and authoritarianism to have a direct impact on measures like the attributes necessary for citizenship and an indirect impact via their effects on perceptions of threats – this is a conservative approach to gauging the effects of perceptions of threats. Table 2 indicates statistical significance from zero, and also whether the differences in the impact of perceptions of the breadth of threats at different levels are statistically significant. All dependent variables are coded from zero to one with the exception of the counts of the breadth of threats at each level (see online appendix for details). For simplicity, we limit our presentation and discussion to the influence of perceived threats.Footnote 107

Table 2 The Effects of Threats on Political Attitudes and Behaviour

gdifferent from global threat at <0.10;ndifferent from national threat at <0.10;cdifferent from community threat at <0.10;pdifferent from personal/family threat at <0.10.

Note: Estimates are from a generalized structural equation model using Stata 13, and assume the count variables – breadth of threats – have Poisson distributions. Statistical significance may not always appear accurate due to rounding. *p < 0.05,#p < 0.10 (two-tailed test).

Table 1 and Figure 3 suggest that global and national threats differ somewhat in their determinants, both from each other and from community and personal threats. The first four columns of results in Table 2 indicate that their effects on attitudes toward citizenship and immigration also differ, but not in the ways that previous research would lead us to expect. Perceptions of global and national threats do not have a more pervasive influence on these attitudes than community or personal threats: four of the eight estimates are statistically significant for global and national threats (p < 0.10), which is no more than for community and personal threats. Neither is it the case that global and national threats are statistically significant for different dependent variables (that is, influence different attitudes in Models 1 to 4 than community or personal threats). The direction of effects is also inconsistent with previous claims about sociotropic threat effects: (1) perceptions of the breadth of global threats have a more consistent impact than perceptions of the breadth of national threats and (2) the direction of effects for perceptions of global threats tends to be opposite to that of community and personal threats. Models 1 to 4 show that perceptions of more global threats are associated with viewing cultural conformity as less important to citizenship and with less concern about immigration, but also with considering attributes such as education and work skills as more important for citizenship.Footnote 108 By contrast, perceptions of more community and personal threats are associated with heightened concerns about the importance of cultural considerations for citizenship and immigration, and with linking immigration with negative economic outcomes and terrorism.Footnote 109 In sum, the first four columns of results in Table 2 show community and personal threats connected with various kinds of antipathy toward immigrants, while global threats are associated with the opposite.

Models 5 to 7 in Table 2 show the estimated relationships between perceived threats and respondents’ willingness to pay £50 more in taxes toward border control, defence and policing – a small increase but one that divided respondents. These are largely national-level issues, and one might therefore expect a more pronounced influence of sociotropic than sub-national threats. To an extent this is borne out in the estimates: the relationships between perceptions of global and national threats are statistically stronger than those for community and personal threats – five of six estimates are significant at p < 0.10 compared to one for sub-national threats (p < 0.10). However, the estimates also show the same contrast as in Models 1 to 4 between the effects of perceptions of global threats and perceptions of the breadth of threats at other levels. Perceptions of global threats are associated with less support for extra taxation to pay for border control and defence, whereas perceptions of national threats are associated with a willingness to pay more for all three forms of security, and community and personal threats are associated with support for extra tax for defence. The fact that there is overlap between the kinds of security threats that are identified at the global and national levels in particular – terrorism, religious extremism, the economic crisis – but that the relationships with other political attitudes vary, indicates, as suggested in our discussion above, that identifying these threats as world problems rather than as national or sub-national threats may represent a different cultural world view, or perhaps that global threats raise different considerations than national or sub-national threats.

The last column of results in Table 2, Model 8 estimates the relationship between breadth of threats and certainty of voting. The estimates contrast with those we found for attitudes toward immigrants and taxation. The theory that threatened individuals are more motivated to vote is confirmed, but the positive effects are confined to sociotropic threats. With voting, global and national threats do not pull in opposite directions; both are associated with greater certainty that a respondent will vote in the next general election, and with significantly greater certainty than where there are perceptions of more personal threats. Indeed, more personal threats are associated with a reduced certainty to vote, suggesting either that personal threats are seen as less likely to be resolved by the national government or that personal travails limit an individual's motivation to vote.

Figure 4 puts these results into substantive perspective by estimating the maximum effects of perceptions of the breadth of threats at different levels on key variables in Table 2. It clearly shows the differences in the influence of perceptions of global and sub-national threats – community-level threats in particular – on attitudes toward immigrants and between perceptions of the breadth of global and national threats on the willingness to pay more in taxes. These differences amount to as much as 30 per cent for the willingness to pay more in tax toward border security: all else equal, this means that whereas perceiving more global threats moves a respondent from tending to support paying more tax for border security to indifference, perceptions of more national security threats moves a respondent from indifference to support for paying more tax. Figure 4 also shows that while the impact of perceptions of community and personal threats varies, it is frequently as large as that from global and national threats.

Fig. 4 Maximum effects of breadth of threats Note: Simulations are with all other variables set at their mean or mode (a white, non-Muslim woman, without higher education).

In addition, Figure 4 illustrates the contrasting impact of perceptions of threats on certainty of voting: global and national threats enhance the certainty of voting by between 0.1 and 0.2, while perceptions of personal threats reduce the certainty of voting by more than 0.1, or 10 per cent of the scale. This implies that perceptions of more global and national threats are associated with both expecting to vote at the next general election and being able to name the party they are going to vote for, whereas respondents who are more personally threatened are also less certain of the party they will vote for.

Another way of gauging the size of these effects is to compare them with the other variables in the model. These calculations show that where the effects of threats are greatest in Figure 4, they are the largest (or among the largest) of any of the variables in the models. For example, the maximum effect of perceptions of the breadth of community threats on attitudes toward cultural convention as a necessity for citizenship is slightly smaller than that of authoritarianism, but is comparable to age and religiosity and greater than any other variable; the maximum effect of perceptions of national threats on the willingness to pay more tax for border control is larger than that of any other variable; the effect of perceptions of global threat on vote intention is larger than for all variables except age.

CONCLUSION

This article has examined public perceptions of security threats at a time when citizens are increasingly expected to play an active role in states’ security apparatuses. Vigilance and engagement can, however, be accompanied by a heightened sense of threat, and democratic citizenship may become strained under such conditions. To address the issue of how to both combat and mitigate perceptions of security threats, we must first understand them. Our argument has been that we do not understand them adequately due in large part to an elite focus in security studies, and a lack of empirical research on public attitudes toward the breadth of security threats at different levels in the political behaviour literature.

We have focused in this article on the breadth of threats perceived by individuals at the global, national, community and personal levels, as well as their origins and consequences. We began by arguing that the predominant focus on the intensity of specific threats of the moment, or extreme stimuli, limits our ability to understand how threatened individuals are (and by what) and whether there is variation in the determinants and effects. This is the first study we know of that examines perceptions of the breadth of threats.

We have shown that members of the British public perceive the most threats at the global level and the least at the community and personal levels, and that the kinds of threats they identify as global or national tend to differ from those they identify as community or personal. We have also demonstrated variation in the origins of perceptions of the breadth of threats, which illustrates the value of our approach. For example, while age consistently elevates perceptions of the breadth of threats at all levels, the influence of authoritarianism is limited to national and sub-national perceptions, while education affects perceptions of the breadth of global but not national-level threats. This suggests that ways to address the insidious effects of authoritarian attitudes toward democracy, such as by a ‘greater appearance of unity and consensus, less display of diversity and airing of differences’,Footnote 110 need the focus to be specifically national and sub-national – stressing unity and consensus beyond the nation will not affect the perceptions of authoritarians.

The effects of threats on attitudes and behaviour have also provided new evidence. While our analysis has confirmed that sociotropic, rather than sub-national, concerns drive vote intention, this frequently observed relationship was shown to be sui generis. We theorized and demonstrated that attitudes with less clear national responsibility and consequences, such as toward immigrants, would show a greater influence on perceptions of community and personal threats. We also showed that perceptions of global threats appear to reflect a different outlook, leading to effects on attitudes that differ from those of community or personal threats in particular, but also from national threats in some areas.

If sociotropic threats are paramount, as previous research has concluded, it implies that ameliorating national threat should be the focus in order to strengthen political tolerance or reduce aggression toward outgroups. But our findings suggest that such a focus would be misplaced – amelioration of threats requires attention to perceptions at the community and personal levels as well. In addition, the differences we have shown between the effects of perceptions of global and national threats indicate that reducing perceptions of global threats is far less important to elements of democratic citizenship such as tolerance – indeed, such a reduction could in fact backfire – than reducing perceptions of national threats. This implies that government attempts to manipulate perceptions of threat in order to sway public attitudes may not be quite as straightforward as previously thought: depiction of the ‘global’ threat of terrorism, for example, would have different consequences than framing terrorism as a threat to the ‘homeland’.

As well as providing new evidence about the origins and consequences of security threats, our findings suggest the need for future research to apply and extend this work. Our research is based on a single country in 2012: the findings require testing at different times and in different countries. Future research should look at specific threats, such as the threat from terrorism or the environment, and at the extent to which the differences we have found in the origins, levels and consequences of threats apply to them. Further extensions of the research should also explore potential moderators of the relationships we have shown for variables like mortality salience and authoritarianism, such as political knowledge, and should examine the intensity as well as the breadth of threats. If we are right about different political outlooks being responsible for perceptions of threats as global rather than national, future research should examine the origins of those outlooks: for example, whether individuals who identify more global threats do so as a consequence of stronger identities as international (rather than national) citizens.

Our findings also have broader implications for future research. For example, we expressed frustration with the notion that the effects of sub-national considerations are ‘sporadic’ and ‘here and there’Footnote 111 and sought a firmer grasp on when they will be influential. Not only are voting intentions unique in the extent to which sociotropic concerns dominate, but when we move away from dependent variables that refer to national consequences (or for which the national government has clear responsibility), sub-national considerations become increasingly powerful. This article also suggests that the level of threat on which researchers choose to focus, for example the physical threat of terrorism,Footnote 112 will affect the findings such that the results cannot be generalized to the perceptions of the national or global threat of terrorism.

Speer writes of how, ‘The terms international, national, and social security have to be used by some securitizing actors to amplify small threats.’Footnote 113 Yet our research suggests that framing security threats as international rather than national may have very different consequences on who is threatened and on public preferences for policies such as spending on security – an insight that also has implications for ongoing efforts to refine and further develop the insights of securitization theory.Footnote 114 Meyer says that ‘variations in threat perceptions can explain some of the variation in policy responses [to terrorism]… within the EU’,Footnote 115 but our research adds an additional layer of nuance: it is not simply variation in how threatened individuals are that matters, but the level at which they perceive those threats. This insight is potentially of direct relevance to policy makers who purport to incorporate citizens’ views into the formulation of national security policy. Equally, from a citizen's perspective, this finding questions the adequacy of the ‘national’ frame and points to the need for more contextualized and less homogenized approaches to understanding the contemporary politics of security threat perceptions.

Footnotes

*

Department of Politics, University of Exeter; Department of Politics and International Studies, University of Warwick (emails: D.P.Stevens@exeter.ac.uk, N.Vaughan-Williams@warwick.ac.uk). Support for this research was provided by the Economic and Social Research Council (grant number ES/J004596/1). Thanks to Susan Banducci and Chris Federico for helpful comments and suggestions. This paper was completed while Stevens was a visiting professor at Australian National University. An online appendix with replication data, supplementary tables and details of the coding of variables is available at http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1017/S0007123414000143.

1 HM Government 2008, 2010.

2 Jarvis and Lister Reference Jarvis and Lister2010, 174. See also Vaughan-Williams Reference Vaughan-Williams2008; Malcolm Reference Malcolm2013.

3 Massumi Reference Massumi2005; McDermott and Zimbardo Reference McDermott and Zimbardo2006.

4 Motyl, Hart, and Pyszczynski Reference Motyl, Hart and Pyszczynski2010; Burke, Kosloff, and Landau Reference Burke, Kosloff and Landau2013.

5 Chalk Reference Chalk1998; Bigo and Tsoukala Reference Bigo and Tsoukala2008; Nacos, Bloch-Elkon, and Shapiro Reference Nacos, Bloch-Elkon and Shapiro2011.

6 Esposito Reference Esposito2011, 16.

9 Merolla, Ramos, and Zechmeister Reference Merolla, Ramos and Zechmeister2007.

10 Sibley, Wilson, and Duckitt Reference Sibley, Wilson and Duckitt2007, 368

12 Sniderman, Hagendoorn, and Prior Reference Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior2004; Huysmans Reference Huysmans2006.

13 Brader, Valentino, and Suhay Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Lahav and Courtemanche Reference Lahav and Courtemanche2012.

15 Buzan and Hansen Reference Buzan and Hansen2009; Peoples and Vaughan-Williams Reference Peoples and Vaughan-Williams2014.

17 Buzan Reference Buzan1991; Buzan, Wæver, and de Wilde Reference Buzan, Wæver and de Wilde1998.

20 Campbell Reference Campbell1998.

21 Buzan, Wæver, and de Wilde Reference Buzan, Wæver and de Wilde1998.

25 Altemeyer Reference Altemeyer1996, 100.

26 Lahav and Courtemanche Reference Lahav and Courtemanche2012.

27 Baldassare and Katz Reference Baldassare and Katz1992.

29 Campbell Reference Campbell2006.

30 Steele and Aronson Reference Steele and Aronson1995; Huguet and Regner Reference Huguet and Regner2007.

31 Gibson and Gouws Reference Gibson and Gouws2003, 198.

33 Feldman and Stenner Reference Feldman and Stenner1997; Feldman Reference Feldman2003; Stenner Reference Stenner2005; Hetherington and Suhay Reference Hetherington and Suhay2011. Stenner distinguishes between ‘normative threat’ – the threat of a changeable society – and ‘perceptions of a dangerous world’. Authoritarians are no more likely to be normatively threatened, she argues, but they are more likely to be generally fearful of ‘disorder’, ‘chaos’ and ‘anarchy’, or a dangerous world (2005, 69–71). Hetherington and Weiler (Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009, 8) also argue that authoritarians are more likely to see threats.

35 Sniderman, Hagendoorn, and Prior Reference Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior2004

36 Huddy et al. Reference Huddy, Feldman, Capelos and Provost2002; Huddy et al. Reference Huddy, Feldman, Taber and Lahav2005. There is disagreement about whether authoritarianism is a personality trait or a predisposition. To ease the flow of discussion, we refer to it as a predisposition from now on.

38 Ridout, Grosse, and Appleton Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008.

39 Lupia and Menning Reference Lupia and Menning2009, 104, italics in original.

40 Focus on extreme stimuli also precludes analysis of the extent to which perceptions of threat change over time.

47 Chanley Reference Chanley1994.

49 Hellwig Reference Hellwig2001; Burden and Mughan Reference Burden and Mughan2003. The causes are related to those usually given for greater national than personal effects – globalization blurs responsibility for the economy, and international economic indicators have become more salient as the news media have given them more attention.

50 Chong, Citrin, and Conley Reference Chong, Citrin and Conley2001, 544.

53 Niemi, Bremer, and Heel Reference Niemi, Bremer and Heel1999.

54 Kam and Nam Reference Kam and Nam2008.

55 Hopkins Reference Hopkins2010.

56 Gartner and Segura Reference Gartner and Segura2000.

57 Glasgow Reference Glasgow2005.

58 Lau and Heldman Reference Lau and Heldman2009, 535.

59 Huddy et al. Reference Huddy, Feldman, Capelos and Provost2002; Josyln and Haider-Markel 2007; Maoz and McCauley Reference Maoz and McCauley2009; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2009. A rare exception is Ridout, Grosse, and Appleton (Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008), who operationalize sociotropic threat as global threat.

60 Chanley Reference Chanley1994.

61 Lee, Dallaire, and Lemyre Reference Lee, Dallaire and Lemyre2009, 435.

62 See also, Ridout, Grosse, and Appleton Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008, 579–80; Reifler, Scotto, and Clarke Reference Reifler, Scotto and Clarke2011.

64 See also, Duckitt, and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2010.

70 Sanquist, Mahy, and Morris Reference Sanquist, Mahy and Morris2008; Green Reference Green2009; Kalkan, Layman, and Uslaner Reference Kalkan, Layman and Uslaner2009; Merolla and Zechmeister Reference Merolla and Zechmeister2009; Aly and Green Reference Aly and Green2010; Croft Reference Croft2012.

74 Altemeyer Reference Altemeyer1996. See also, Sibley, Wilson and Duckitt Reference Sibley, Wilson and Duckitt2007.

75 Stenner (Reference Stenner2005, 69) refers to ‘perceptions of a dangerous world’ – ‘a persistent fear of societal “disorder”, “chaos”, and “anarchy”’ (italics in original) – as distinct from normative threat. Our measure of security threats (see below) appears to be broader than these perceptions.

76 Hetherington and Weiler Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009.

77 Ridout, Grosse, and Appleton Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008.

78 Nacos, Bloch-Elkon, and Shapiro Reference Nacos, Bloch-Elkon and Shapiro2011.

79 Hurwitz and Peffley Reference Hurwitz and Peffley1997; Gilliam and Iyengar Reference Gilliam and Iyengar2000.

84 Gillespie and O'Loughlin Reference Gillespie and O'Loughlin2009. The booster sample was to allow examination of the relationship between threat perceptions and the attitudes of British Muslims compared to other groups, but that is not the focus of this article. The survey was administered by ICM and the sample was drawn from its online panel. Internet surveys from online panels remain an area of contention, with some prominent researchers sanguine about them (Sanders et al. Reference Sanders, Clarke, Stewart and Whiteley2007), while others argue that they produce less accurate surveys than random probability samples (Yeager et al. Reference Yeager, Krosnick, Chang, Javitz, Levendusky, Simpser and Wang2011).

85 As tends to be the case in surveys, our sample contained a lower proportion of non-voters than in the population, but this proportion compares favourably to the face-to-face British Election Study, which contained fewer non-voters (22 per cent).

86 We conducted ten ninety-minute mini-focus groups of three people prior to the survey, which varied by age/ life stage, sex and religion (see the online appendix for details) in April 2012, in which we encouraged respondents to discuss matters of security in their own language.

87 An alternative approach would have been open-ended questions about perceptions of threats, but we preferred a list format for three reasons. First, we were interested in the combination of threats that both may occur to respondents spontaneously and also be regarded as threats when prompted to think about them (that is, they may be further back in consciousness). Secondly, some of the issues listed, such as immigration and religious extremism, were sensitive, and respondents may be less willing to identify them themselves than when looking at a pre-defined set of issues. Thirdly, we felt there was a greater danger of conflating the ability to articulate threats with genuine perceptions of the breadth of security threats with an open-ended question.

88 It is possible that people think of their community as the nation or the world, but we consider it unlikely here. The survey asked about the ‘community in which you live’. Evidence from the focus groups and the survey indicates very strongly that ‘community’ has sub-national, indeed local, connotations for most people.

89 Asking about the intensity of each threat would have necessitated up to eighty-eight (22 × 4) additional questions, leading to respondent fatigue and questionable data quality. Our priority in the survey was to capture perceptions of the breadth of threats with a large range of potential security threats. In our qualitative work, we allowed participants to compare and contrast issues, to rank them in terms of the immediacy of the threat posed to them in everyday life, and to discuss in depth an issue they felt particularly threatened by (for more on how we tackled this issue and our findings, see Vaughan-Williams and Stevens ND). We also deliberately ignore possible relationships between threats; for example, whether perceptions of threat from the economic crisis are related to heightened perceptions of threat from crime, which are beyond the scope of this article. For our purposes, these are two distinct threats.

90 Feldman Reference Feldman2003.

91 Half the survey sample was asked these questions at the beginning of the survey and the other half at the end, in order to guard against the possibility that the content of the survey would raise mortality salience or that asking about mortality at the outset of the survey would affect the answers to other questions. Neither appears to have been the case (a chi-square test of the mortality salience index by question order is statistically insignificant).

92 For example, Feldman Reference Feldman2003; Stenner Reference Stenner2005; Hetherington and Weiler Reference Hetherington and Weiler2009.

93 Similarly, we expect that white ethnicity is unlikely to affect perceptions of threats at the global or national level, but could at the community or personal levels, for example as a result of ‘white flight’ to apparently safer neighbourhoods. Akaike and Bayesian Information Criteria confirm that models without these restrictions have an inferior fit to the data. The operationalization of white ethnicity and the other control variables of sex, age, Muslim religion and religiosity are described in the online appendix.

95 Thus the x-axes from one to seven threats extend to roughly the average number of threats identified at the global level but to the outer edges of the distribution for community and personal threats.

96 There is somewhat more change in the rankings of community and personal threats, but the number of respondents identifying more than two threats at these levels becomes small – that is, the greater instability appears to be due to small numbers of respondents.

98 We also asked about threats in the future. These differed little in number or content from those that respondents viewed as current threats, although there was a slight tendency to identify more threats in the future.

99 All estimates assume Poisson distributions for the breadth of threats, using Stata 13's gsem command, because they are counts that are bounded at zero with a long right-hand tail. gsem models do not provide estimates of model fit (models with different restrictions can be compared – see footnote 93). However, alternative models that do not assume Poisson distributions, but that transform the counts with natural logs, or that ignore their skewness altogether, provide similar results with identical substantive implications. More importantly, they also give estimates of model fit, and they indicate excellent fit to the data. Results of these models are available from the authors on request.

100 The effect does not quite reach conventional levels of statistical significance at the community level (p = 0.07). Wald tests of the difference in coefficients also show that the size of effects is greater on global and national threats (at p < 0.05) than on community threats, and greater for global than personal threats. However, fewer threats are identified at the community and personal levels.

102 Burke, Kosloff, and Landau Reference Burke, Kosloff and Landau2013.

103 One way of looking at this is to separate respondents who named the ‘physical’ dangers of knife crime and burglary from those who did not. When we did this, the difference in the effects of mortality salience between the two groups of respondents was not statistically significant; indeed, the coefficient was larger for respondents who did not identify these dangers, which favours this explanation.

104 Ridout, Grosse, and Appleton Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008.

105 Stenner Reference Stenner2005, 32.

106 None of these results or conclusions is changed by additional controls for left-right attitudes as represented by party identification. This analysis is available from the authors on request.

107 All other estimates from the models are included in the online appendix.

108 These relationships are not a function of collinearity: we examined the bivariate relationships between global threat and these variables, and they are consistent with the results in Table 2. Because Table 1 showed that Muslims see fewer global threats, we also re-estimated the models split by Muslims and non-Muslims, but they do not differ. Finally, we looked at the data used by Ridout, Grosse, and Appleton (Reference Ridout, Grosse and Appleton2008) and compared the impact of global and national threats. They show effects that are opposite in sign and consistent with our results.

109 While the individual coefficients for the effects of community and personal threats are not always statistically significantly different from zero, Wald tests (shown in the table) indicate that they are frequently statistically significantly different from the effects of global and national threats.

110 Stenner Reference Stenner2005, 333, italics in original.

111 Lau and Heldman Reference Lau and Heldman2009, 535.

112 Hetherington and Suhay Reference Hetherington and Suhay2011.

113 Speer Reference Speer2000, 266.

115 Meyer Reference Meyer2009, 664.

References

Adorno, Theodor, Frenkel-Brunswick, Else, Levison, Daniel Sanford, R. Nevitt. 1950. The Authoritarian Personality. New York: Harper and Brothers.Google Scholar
Altemeyer, Bob. 1996. The Authoritarian Specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Aly, Anne Green, Lelia. 2010. Fear, Anxiety and the State of Terror. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 33:268281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldassare, Mark Katz, Cheryl. 1992. The Personal Threat of Environmental Problems as Predictor of Environmental Practices. Environment and Behavior 24:602616.Google Scholar
Balzacq, Thierry, ed. 2011. Securitization Theory: How Security Problems Emerge and Dissolve. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bigo, Didier Tsoukala, Anastassia, eds. 2008. Terror, Insecurity and Liberty: Illiberal Practices of Liberal Regimes after 9/11. London and New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, Ken. 1991. Security and Emancipation. Review of International Studies 17:313326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brader, Ted, Valentino, Nicholas Suhay, Elizabeth. 2008. What Triggers Public Opposition to Immigration? Anxiety, Group Cues, and Immigration Threat. American Journal of Political Science 52:959978.Google Scholar
Burden, Barry Mughan, Anthony. 2003. The International Economy and Presidential Approval. Public Opinion Quarterly 67:555578.Google Scholar
Burke, Brian, Kosloff, Spee Landau, Mark. 2013. Death Goes to the Polls: A Meta-Analysis of Mortality Salience Effects on Political Attitudes. Political Psychology 34:183200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buzan, Barry. 1991. People, States and Fear: An Agenda for International Studies in the Post-Cold War Era, 2nd edition. Boulder: Lynne Rienner.Google Scholar
Buzan, Barry Hansen, Lene. 2009. The Evolution of International Security Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Buzan, Barry, Wæver, Ole de Wilde, Jaap. 1998. Security: A New Framework for Analysis. London: Lynne Rienner.Google Scholar
Campbell, David. 1998. Writing Security: United States Foreign Policy and the Politics of Identity. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, David. 2006. Religious ‘Threat’ in Contemporary Presidential Elections. Journal of Politics 68:104115.Google Scholar
Canetti-Nisim, Daphna, Halperin, Eran, Hobfoll, Stevan, Shapira, Oren Hirsch-Hoefler, Sivan. 2009. Authoritarianism, Perceived Threat and Exclusionism on the Eve of the Disengagement: Evidence from Gaza. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 33:463474.Google Scholar
Chalk, Peter. 1998. The Response to Terrorism as a Threat to Liberal Democracy. Australian Journal of Politics and History 44:373388.Google Scholar
Chanley, Virginia. 1994. Commitment to Political Tolerance: Situational and Activity-Based Differences. Political Psychology 16:343363.Google Scholar
Chong, Dennis, Citrin, Jack Conley, Patricia. 2001. When Self-Interest Matters. Political Psychology 22:541570.Google Scholar
Clarke, Harold, Sanders, David, Stewart, Marianne Whiteley, Paul. 2004. Political Choice in Britain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, Stuart. 2012. Securitizing Islam: Identity and the Search for Security. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalby, Simon. 2002. Environmental Security. Minneapolis and London: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Davies, Paul, Steele, Claude Markus, Hazel. 2008. A Nation Challenged: The Impact of Foreign Threat on America's Tolerance for Diversity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:308318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, Darren Silver, Brian. 2004. Civil Liberties vs. Security: Public Opinion in the Context of the Terrorist Attacks on America. American Journal of Political Science 48:2846.Google Scholar
Duckitt, John Sibley, Chris. 2010. Personality, Ideology, Prejudice, and Politics: A Dual-Process Motivational Model. Journal of Personality 78:18611894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esposito, Roberto. 2011. Immunitas: The Protection and Negation of Life. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Feldman, Stanley. 2003. Enforcing Social Conformity: A Theory of Authoritarianism. Political Psychology 24:4174.Google Scholar
Feldman, Stanley Stenner, Karen. 1997. Perceived Threat and Authoritarianism. Political Psychology 18:741770.Google Scholar
Fischer, Peter, Fischer, Julia, Frey, Dieter, Such, Michelle, Smyth, Madlein, Test, Michelle Kastenmuller, Andreas. 2010. Causal Evidence that Terrorism Salience Increases Authoritarian Parenting Practices. Social Psychology 41:246254.Google Scholar
Fromm, Erich. 1941. Escape from Freedom. New York: Owl, Rinehart and Winston, Inc.Google Scholar
Gartner, Scott Sigmund Segura, Gary. 2000. Race, Casualties, and Opinion in the Vietnam War. Journal of Politics 62:115146.Google Scholar
Gibson, James. 1986. Pluralistic Intolerance in America: A Reconsideration. American Politics Research 14:267293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James Gouws, Amanda. 2003. Overcoming Intolerance in South Africa: Experiments in Democratic Persuasion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gillespie, Marie O'Loughlin, Ben. 2009. News Media, Threats and Insecurities: An Ethnographic Approach. Cambridge Review of International Affairs 22:667685.Google Scholar
Gilliam, Frank Iyengar, Shanto. 2000. Prime Suspects: The Influence of Television News on the Viewing Public. American Journal of Political Science 44:560573.Google Scholar
Glasgow, Garrett. 2005. Evidence of Group-Based Economic Voting: NAFTA and Union Households in the 1992 Presidential Election. Political Research Quarterly 58:427434.Google Scholar
Green, Eva. 2009. Who Can Enter? A Multilevel Analysis on Public Support for Immigration Criteria across 20 European Countries. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 12:4160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, Jeff, Simon, Linda, Harmon-Jones, Eddie, Solomon, Sheldon, Pyszczynski, Tom Lyon, Deborah. 1995. Explanations for Mortality Salience Effects: Terror Management, Value Accessibility, or Worrisome Thoughts? European Journal of Social Psychology 25:417433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, Jeff, Pyszczynski, Tom, Solomon, Sheldon, Rosenblatt, Abram, Veeder, Mitchel, Kirkland, Shari Lyon, Deborah. 1990. Evidence for Terror Management Theory II: The Effects of Mortality Salience on Reactions to those Who Threaten or Bolster the Cultural Worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58:308318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, Jeff, Pyszczynski, Tom, Solomon, Sheldon, Simon, Linda Breus, Michael. 1994. The Role of Consciousness and Accessibility of Death-Related Thoughts in Mortality Salience Effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67:627637.Google Scholar
Hellwig, Timothy. 2001. Interdependence, Government Constraints, and Economic Voting. Journal of Politics 63:11411162.Google Scholar
Hetherington, Marc Suhay, Elizabeth. 2011. Authoritarianism, Threat, and Americans’ Support for the War on Terror. American Journal of Political Science 55:546560.Google Scholar
Hetherington, Marc Weiler, Jonathan. 2009. Authoritarianism and Polarization in American Politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heymann, Philip. 1998. Terrorism and America: A Commonsense Strategy for a Democratic Society. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
HM Government. 2008. The National Security Strategy of the United Kingdom: Security in an Interdependent World. Presented to the UK Parliament by the prime minister, London.Google Scholar
HM Government. 2010. A Strong Britain in an Age of Uncertainty: The National Security Strategy. Presented to the UK Parliament by the prime minister, London.Google Scholar
Hopkins, Daniel. 2010. Politicized Places: Explaining Where and When Immigrants Provoke Local Opposition. American Political Science Review 104:4060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, Feldman, Stanley, Taber, Charles Lahav, Gallya. 2005. Threat, Anxiety, and Support of Antiterrorism Policies. American Journal of Political Science 49:593608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, Feldman, Stanley, Capelos, Theresa Provost, Colin. 2002. The Consequences of Terrorism: Disentangling the Effects of Personal and National Threat. Political Psychology 23:485509.Google Scholar
Huguet, Pascal Regner, Isabelle. 2007. Stereotype Threat among Schoolgirls in Quasi-Ordinary Classroom Circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology 99:545560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurwitz, Jon Peffley, Mark. 1997. Public Perceptions of Race and Crime: The Role of Racial Stereotypes. American Journal of Political Science 41:375401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huysmans, Jef. 2006. The Politics of Insecurity: Fear, Migration and Asylum in the EU. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jarvis, Lee Lister, Michael. 2010. Stakeholder Security: The New Western Way of Counter-Terrorism? Contemporary Politics 16:173188.Google Scholar
Jarvis, Lee Lister, Michael. 2013. Disconnected Citizenship? The Impacts of Anti-Terrorism Policy on Citizenship in the UK. Political Studies 61:656675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, Ron, Pattie, Charles, Dorling, Daniel, McAllister, Iain, Tunstall, Helena Rossiter, David. 2000. Local Context, Retrospective Economic Evaluations, and Voting: The 1997 General Election in England and Wales. Political Behavior 22:121143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K., Johnston, Ron Pattie, Charles. 1992. People, Places, and Regions: Exploring the Use of Multi-Level Modelling in the Analysis of Electoral Data. British Journal of Political Science 22:343380.Google Scholar
Joslyn, Mark Haider-Markel, Donald. 2007. Sociotropic Concerns and Support for Counterterrorism Policies. Social Science Quarterly 88:306319.Google Scholar
Kahan, Dan. 2012. Cultural Cognition as a Conception of the Cultural Theory of Risk. In Handbook of Risk Theory: Epistemology, Decision Theory, Ethics and Social Implications of Risk, edited by Sabine Roeser, Rafaela Hillerbrand, Per Sandin and Martin Peterson, 752760. London: Springer.Google Scholar
Kahan, Dan Braman, Donald. 2006. Cultural Cognition and Public Policy. Yale Law and Policy Review 149:147170.Google Scholar
Kalkan, Kerem Ozan, Layman, Geoffrey Uslaner, Eric. 2009. ‘Bands of Others’? Attitudes Toward Muslims in Contemporary American Society. Journal of Politics 71:847862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kam, Cindy Nam, Yunju. 2008. Reaching Out or Pulling Back: Macroeconomic Conditions and Public Support for Social Welfare Spending. Political Behavior 30:223258.Google Scholar
Kerwin, Donald. 2005. The Use and Misuse of ‘National Security’ Rationale in Crafting U.S. Refugee and Immigration Policies. International Journal of Refugee Law 17:749763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinder, Donald Roderick Kiewiet, D.. 1981. Sociotropic Politics: The American Case. British Journal of Political Science 11:129161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinder, Donald Sears, David. 1981. Prejudice and Politics: Symbolic Racism Versus Racial Threat to the Good Life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40:414431.Google Scholar
Lahav, Gallya Courtemanche, Marie. 2012. The Ideological Effects of Framing Threat on Immigration and Civil Liberties. Political Behavior 34:477505.Google Scholar
Landau, Mark, Solomon, Sheldon, Greenberg, Jeff, Cohen, Florette, Pyszczynski, Tom, Arndt, Jamie, Miller, Claude, Ogilvie, Daniel Cook, Alison. 2004. Deliver us from Evil: The Effects of Mortality Salience and Reminders of 9/11 on Support for President George W. Bush. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30:11361150.Google Scholar
Lau, Richard Heldman, Caroline. 2009. Self-Interest, Symbolic Attitudes, and Support for Public Policy: A Multilevel Analysis. Political Psychology 30:513537.Google Scholar
Lavine, Howard, Sullivan, John, Borgida, Eugene Thomsen, Cynthia. 1996. The Relationship of National and Personal Issue Salience to Attitude Accessibility on Foreign and Domestic Policy Issues. Political Psychology 17:293316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavine, Howard, Burgess, Diana, Snyder, Mark, Transue, John, Sullivan, John, Haney, Beth Wagner, Stephen. 1999. Threat, Authoritarianism, and Voting: An Investigation of Personality and Persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25:337347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Jennifer, Dallaire, Christine Lemyre, Louise. 2009. Qualitative Analysis of Cognitive and Contextual Determinants of Canadians’ Individual Response to Terrorism. Health, Risk & Society 11:431450.Google Scholar
Levinson, Daniel. 1949. An Approach to the Theory and Measurement of Ethnocentric Ideology. Journal of Psychology 28:1939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupia, Arthur Menning, Jesse. 2009. When Can Politicians Scare Citizens into Supporting Bad Policies? American Journal of Political Science 53:90106.Google Scholar
Malcolm, James. 2013. Project Argus and the Resilient Citizen. Politics 33:311321.Google Scholar
Maoz, Ifat McCauley, Clark. 2009. Threat Perceptions and Feelings as Predictors of Jewish-Israeli Support for Compromise with Palestinians. Journal of Peace Research 46:525539.Google Scholar
Marcus, George, Sullivan, John, Theiss-Morse, Elizabeth Wood, Sandra. 1995. With Malice Toward Some: How People Make Civil Liberties Judgments. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Massumi, Brian. 2005. Fear (The Spectrum said). Positions 13:3148.Google Scholar
McDonald, Matt. 2008. Securitization and the Construction of Security. European Journal of International Relations 14:563587.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose Zimbardo, Philip. 2006. The Psychological Consequences of Terrorist Alerts. In Psychology of Terrorism, edited by Bruce Bongar, Lisa Brown, Larry Beutler, James Breckenridge, and Philip Zimbardo, 357372. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, Holly, Greenberg, Jeff, Arndt, Jamie, Lieberman, Joel, Soloman, Sheldon, Simon, Linda Pyszczynski, Tom. 1998. Terror Management and Aggression: Evidence that Mortality Salience Motivates Aggression Against World View-Threatening Others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74:590605.Google Scholar
Merolla, Jennifer Zechmeister, Elizabeth. 2009. Terrorist Threat, Leadership, and the Vote: Evidence from Three Experiments. Political Behavior 31:575601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merolla, Jennifer, Ramos, Jennifer Zechmeister, Elizabeth. 2007. Crisis, Charisma, and Consequences: Evidence from the 2004 U.S. Presidential Election. Journal of Politics 69:3042.Google Scholar
Meyer, Christoph. 2009. International Terrorism as a Force of Homogenization? A Constructivist Approach to Understanding Cross-National Threat Perceptions and Responses. Cambridge Review of International Affairs 22:647666.Google Scholar
Motyl, Matt, Hart, Joshua Pyszczynski, Tom. 2010. When Animals Attack: The Effects of Mortality Salience, Infrahumanization of Violence, and Authoritarianism on Support for War. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46:200203.Google Scholar
Mutz, Diana. 1992. Mass Media and the Depoliticization of Personal Experience. American Journal of Political Science 36:483508.Google Scholar
Nacos, Brigitte, Bloch-Elkon, Yaeli Shapiro, Robert. 2011. Selling Fear: Counterterrorism, the Media, and Public Opinion. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Neal, Andrew. 2009. Exceptionalism and the Politics of Counter-Terrorism: Liberty, Security and the War on Terror. London and New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, Richard, Bremer, John Heel, Michael. 1999. Determinants of State Economic Perceptions. Political Behavior 21:175193.Google Scholar
Peoples, Columba Vaughan-Williams, Nick. 2014. Critical Security Studies: An Introduction, 2nd edition. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pyszczynski, Tom, Solomon, Sheldon Greenberg, Jeff. 2002. In the Wake of 9/11: The Psychology of Terror. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Reifler, Jason, Scotto, Thomas Clarke, Harold. 2011. Foreign Policy Beliefs in Contemporary Britain: Structure and Relevance. International Studies Quarterly 55:245266.Google Scholar
Reijerse, Arjan, Van Acker, Kaat, Vanbeselaere, Norbert, Phalet, Karen Duriez, Bart. 2013. Beyond the Ethnic-Civic Dichotomy: Cultural Citizenship as a New Way of Excluding Immigrants. Political Psychology 34:611630.Google Scholar
Ridout, Travis, Grosse, Ashley Appleton, Andrew. 2008. News Media Use and Americans’ Perceptions of Global Threat. British Journal of Political Science 38:575593.Google Scholar
Rubin, G. James, Brewin, Chris, Greenberg, Neil, Simpson, John Wessely, Simon. 2005. Psychological and Behavioural Reactions to the Bombings in London on 7 July 2005: Cross Sectional Survey of a Representative Sample of Londoners. BMJ 331:606611.Google Scholar
Sanders, David, Clarke, Harold, Stewart, Marianne Whiteley, Paul. 2007. Does Mode Matter for Modelling Political Choice? Evidence from the 2005 British Election Study. Political Analysis 15:257285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanquist, Thomas, Mahy, Heidi Morris, Frederic. 2008. An Exploratory Risk Perception Study of Attitudes Toward Homeland Security Systems. Risk Analysis 28:11251133.Google Scholar
Schildkraut, Deborah. 2009. The Dynamics of Public Opinion on Ethnic Profiling After 9/11: Results from a Survey Experiment. American Behavioral Scientist 53:6179.Google Scholar
Schmiel, Jeff, Simon, Linda, Greenberg, Jeff, Pyszczynski, Tom, Sheldon, Solomon, Waxmonsky, Jeannette Arndt, Jamie. 1999. Stereotypes and Terror Management: Evidence that Mortality Salience Enhances Stereotypic Thinking and Preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77:905926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sibley, Chris, Wilson, Marc Duckitt, John. 2007. Effects of Dangerous and Competitive Worldviews on Right-Wing Authoritarianism and Social Dominance Orientation over a Five-Month Period. Political Psychology 28:357371.Google Scholar
Sniderman, Paul, Hagendoorn, Louk Prior, Markus. 2004. Predisposing Factors and Situational Triggers: Exclusionary Reactions to Immigrant Minorities. American Political Science Review 98:3549.Google Scholar
Sniderman, Paul, Tetlock, Philip, Glaser, James, Green, Donald Hout, Michael. 1989. Principled Tolerance and the American Mass Public. British Journal of Political Science 19:2545.Google Scholar
Speer, David. 2000. Redefining Borders: The Challenges of Cybercrime. Crime, Law & Social Change 34:259273.Google Scholar
Steele, Claude Aronson, Joshua. 1995. Stereotype Threat and the Intellectual Test Performance of African-Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69:797811.Google Scholar
Stenner, Karen. 2005. The Authoritarian Dynamic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stouffer, Samuel. 1954. Communism, Conformity and Civil Liberties. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Vaughan-Williams, Nick. 2008. Borderwork Beyond Inside/Outside? Frontex, the Citizen-Detective and the War on Terror. Space and Polity 12:6379.Google Scholar
Waltz, Kenneth. 1979. Theory of International Politics. Boston, MA: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Yeager, David, Krosnick, Jon, Chang, Linchiat, Javitz, Harold, Levendusky, Matthew, Simpser, Alberto Wang, Rui. 2011. Comparing the Accuracy of RDD Telephone Surveys and Internet Surveys Conducted with Probability and Non-Probability Samples. Public Opinion Quarterly 75:709747.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 The structural equation models Note: Figure 1 presents the basic approach to modelling without showing arrows for each individual relationship estimated. In the models, Education is specified as an influence on global and national threat only, and White on community and personal threat only.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Major threats at the global, national, community and individual levels

Figure 2

Table 1 Influences on Global, National, Community and Personal/ Family Threats

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Maximum effects of main influences on breadth of threats Note: Simulations are with all other variables set at their mean or mode (a white, non-Muslim woman, without higher education).

Figure 4

Table 2 The Effects of Threats on Political Attitudes and Behaviour

Figure 5

Fig. 4 Maximum effects of breadth of threats Note: Simulations are with all other variables set at their mean or mode (a white, non-Muslim woman, without higher education).

Supplementary material: PDF

Stevens and Vaughan-Williams Supplementary Material

Appendix

Download Stevens and Vaughan-Williams Supplementary Material(PDF)
PDF 152 KB