Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T12:52:34.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prosodic phonology and phonetics*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2008

John J. Ohala
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Haruko Kawasaki
Affiliation:
M.I.T.

Extract

Our colleague Charles Fillmore invented the following parable (personal communication) to characterise the two dominant ways linguists attempt to solve problems of language structure and behaviour.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, G. D. (1972). The location of rhythmic stress beats in English. I & II. Language and Speech 15. 72100; 179195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, W. S. (1973). Accent and rhythm. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bannert, R. & Bredvad-Jensen, A-C. (1977). Temporal organization of Swedish tonal accents: the effect of vowel duration. Working Papers in Linguistics, Lund University 10. 136.Google Scholar
Bannert, R. & Bredvad-Jensen, A-C. (1977). Temporal organization of Swedish tonal accents: the effect of vowel duration in the Gotland dialect. Working Papers in Linguistics, Lund University 15. 133138.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. & Keyser, S. J. (1983). CV phonology: a generative theory of the syllable. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cutting, J. (1975). Predicting initial cluster frequencies by phonetic difference. Status Report on Speech Research (Haskins Laboratories) SR-42/43. 233239.Google Scholar
Eriksson, Y. (1973). Preliminary evidence of syllable locked temporal control of Fo. Quarterly Progress and Status Reports (Speech Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm) 2–3/1973. 2330.Google Scholar
Eriksson, Y. & Alstermark, M. (1972). Fundamental frequency correlates of the grave word accent in Swedish: the effect of vowel duration. Quarterly Progress and Status Reports (Speech Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm) 2–3/1972. 5360.Google Scholar
Foley, J. (1977). Foundations of theoretical phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, C. A. (1979). ‘Perceptual centers’ in speech production and perception. Perception and Psychophysics 25. 375388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujimura, O., Macchi, M. J. & Streeter, L. A. (1978). Perception of stop consonants with conflicting transitional cues: a cross-linguistic study. Language and Speech 21. 337346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, J. A. (1976). Autosegmental phonology. Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Halle, M. & Vergnaud, J. -R. (1980). Three-dimensional phonology. Journal of Linguistic Research 4. 83105.Google Scholar
Hankamer, J. & Aissen, J. (1974). The sonority hierarchy. In Bruck, A., Fox, R., & Galy, M. La (eds.) Papers from the Parasession on Natural Phonology, Chicago Linguistic Society. 131145.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1981). A metrical theory of stress rules. Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Hooper, J. (1976). An introduction to natural generative phonology. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Householder, F. W. Jr. (1956). Unreleased PTK in American English. In Halle, M., Lunt, H. G., McLean, H. & van Schooneveld, C. H. (eds.) For Roman Jakobson. The Hague: Mouton. 235244.Google Scholar
Hudson, G. (1982). Arabic noncatenative morphology without tiers. Paper presented at Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, San Diego.Google Scholar
Javkin, H. (1979). Phonetic universals and phonological change. (Report of the Phonology Laboratory 4). Berkeley: Phonology Laboratory.Google Scholar
Kahn, D. (1976). Syllable-based generalizations in English phonology. Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Kawasaki, H. (1982). An acoustical basis for universal constraints on sound sequences. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Kawasaki, H. & Ohala, J. J. (1981). Acoustic basis for universal constraints on phoneme combinations. JASA 70. 41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kučera, H. & Monroe, G. K. (1968). A comparative quantitative phonology of Russian, Czech, and German. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Liberman, M. (1975). The intonational system of English. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Liberman, M. & Prince, A. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm. LI 8. 249336.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1975). Some temporal regularities of spoken Swedish. In Fant, G. & Tatham, M. A. A. (eds.) Auditory analysis and perception of speech. London: Academic Press. 387396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindblom, B. & Rapp, K. (1973). Some temporal regularities of spoken Swedish. Papers from the Institute of Linguistics, University of Stockholm 21.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B., Lyberg, B. & Holmgren, K. (1976). Durational patterns of Swedish phonology: do they reflect short-term motor memory processes? Stockholm: ms.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1979). On stress and syllabification. LI 10. 443466.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1981). A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology. LI 12. 373418.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1982). Prosodic structure and expletive infixation. Lg 58. 574590.Google Scholar
MacKay, D. G. (1972). The structure of words and syllables: evidence from errors in speech. Cognitive Psychology 3. 210227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malécot, A. (1960). Vowel nasality as a distinctive feature in American English. Lg 36. 222229.Google Scholar
Marcus, S. M. (1981). Acoustic determinants of perceptual center (P-center) location. Perception and Psychophysics 30. 247256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michailovsky, B. (1975). On some Tibeto-Burman sound changes. Proceedings, Annual Meeting, Berkeley Linguistics Society I. 322332.Google Scholar
Morton, J., Marcus, S. & Frankish, C. (1976). Perceptual centers (P-centers). Psychological Review 83. 405408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nabelek, I. & Hirsh, I. J. (1969). On the discrimination of frequency transitions. JASA 45. 15101519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohala, J. J. (1975). Phonetic explanations for nasal sound patterns. In Ferguson, C. A., Hyman, L. M. & Ohala, J. J. (eds.) Nasálfest: papers from a symposium on nasals and nasalization. Stanford: Language Universals Project. 289316.Google Scholar
Ohala, J. J. (1983). The direction of sound change. In Cohen, A. & van den Broecke, M. P. R. (eds.) Abstracts of the xoth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Dordrecht: Foris. 253258.Google Scholar
Ohala, J. J. (1984). The phonological end justifies any means. In Hattori, S. & Inoue, K. (eds.) Proceedings of the 13th International Congress of Linguists, Tokyo. Tokyo: ICL Editorial Committee. 232243.Google Scholar
Ohala, J. J., Amador, M., Araujo, L., Pearson, S. & Peet, M. (1984). Use of synthetic speech parameters to estimate success of word recognition. JASA 75. S93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollack, I. (1968). Detection of rate of change of auditory frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77. 535541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapp, K. (1971). A study of syllable timing. Quarterly Progress and Status Reports (Speech Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm) 1/1971. 14–;19.Google Scholar
Repp, B. H. (1978). Perceptual integration and differentiation of spectral cues for intervocalic stop consonants. Perception and Psychophysics 24. 471485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saporta, S. (1955). Frequency of consonant clusters. Lg 31. 2530.Google Scholar
Schouten, M. E. H. & Pols, L. C. W. (1983). Perception of plosive consonants. In van den Broecke, M., van Heuven, V. & Zonneveld, W. (eds.) Sound structures: studies for Antonie Cohen. Dordrecht: Foris. 227243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, E. (1980). The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. LI 11. 563605.Google Scholar
Steriade, D. (1982). Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. (1971). The role of rapid spectrum changes in the production and perception of speech. In Hammerich, L. L., Jakobson, R. & Zwirner, E. (eds.) Form and substance: phonetic and linguistic papers presented to Eli Fischer-Jørgensen. Copenhagen: Akademisk Forlag. 95101.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. (1980). Discussion during symposium on phonetic universals in phonological systems and their explanation. In Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Vol. 3. Copenhagen: Institute of Phonetics. 185186.Google Scholar
Streeter, L. A. & Nigro, G. N. (1979). The role of medial consonant transitions in word perception. JASA 65. 15331541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuller, B., Kelso, J. A. S. & Harris, K. S. (1982). Interarticular phasing as an index of temporal regularity in speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 8. 460472.Google Scholar
Wang, W. S.-Y. (1959). Transition and release as perceptual cues for final plosives. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 2. 6673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, W. S.-Y. & Crawford, J. (1960). Frequency studies of English consonants. Language and Speech 3. 131139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, W. D. (1874). The relation of vowel and consonant. In Whitney, W. D.. Oriental and linguistic studies. Second series. New York: Scribner, Armstrong & Co. 277300.Google Scholar