Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T16:16:51.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attentional switching forms a genetic link between attention problems and autistic traits in adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2012

T. J. C. Polderman*
Affiliation:
Complex Trait Genetics, Department of Functional Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam (NCA), VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands
R. A. Hoekstra
Affiliation:
Department of Life Sciences, Faculty of Science, The Open University, Milton Keynes, UK
A. A. E. Vinkhuyzen
Affiliation:
The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, St Lucia, QLD, Australia
P. F. Sullivan
Affiliation:
Department of Genetics, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
S. van der Sluis
Affiliation:
Complex Trait Genetics, Department of Functional Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam (NCA), VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands Functional Genomics Section, Department of Clinical Genetics, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
D. Posthuma
Affiliation:
Complex Trait Genetics, Department of Functional Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam (NCA), VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands Medical Genomics Section, Department of Clinical Genetics, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Erasmus University Medical Center/Sophia Children's Hospital, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: T. J. C. Polderman, Ph.D., Complex Trait Genetics, Department of Functional Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam (NCA), VU University Amsterdam, De Boelelaan 1085, 1081 HV, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. (Email: tinca.polderman@vu.nl)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and autistic traits often occur together. The pattern and etiology of co-occurrence are largely unknown, particularly in adults. This study investigated the co-occurrence between both traits in detail, and subsequently examined the etiology of the co-occurrence, using two independent adult population samples.

Method

Data on ADHD traits (Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity) were collected in a population sample (S1, n = 559) of unrelated individuals. Data on Attention Problems (AP) were collected in a population-based family sample of twins and siblings (S2, n = 560). In both samples five dimensions of autistic traits were assessed (social skills, routine, attentional switching, imagination, patterns).

Results

Hyperactive traits (S1) did not correlate substantially with the autistic trait dimensions. For Inattention (S1) and AP (S2), the correlations with the autistic trait dimensions were low, apart from a prominent correlation with the attentional switching scale (0.47 and 0.32 respectively). Analyses in the genetically informative S2 revealed that this association could be explained by a shared genetic factor.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that the co-occurrence of ADHD traits and autistic traits in adults is not determined by problems with hyperactivity, social skills, imagination or routine preferences. Instead, the association between those traits is due primarily to shared attention-related problems (inattention and attentional switching capacity). As the etiology of this association is purely genetic, biological pathways involving attentional control could be a promising focus of future studies aimed at unraveling the genetic causes of these disorders.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence . The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are two conditions that develop during childhood, but are also common conditions in adult psychiatric practice. ADHD is characterized by developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity whereas ASD is characterized by impaired communication and social skills, repetitive behavior and restricted interests. About 4.5% of adults are diagnosed with ADHD (Simon et al. Reference Simon, Czobor, Balint, Meszaros and Bitter2009), and the first prevalence study for ASD in adults suggests that about 1% of the population meet diagnostic criteria (Brugha et al. Reference Brugha, McManus, Bankart, Scott, Purdon, Smith, Bebbington, Jenkins and Meltzer2011). Of note, to be diagnosed as an adult for ADHD or ASD, part of the symptoms have to be recalled as having been present during childhood. ADHD and ASD often co-occur, with approximately 30–50% of adults diagnosed with ASD also meeting criteria for ADHD (Hofvander et al. Reference Hofvander, Delorme, Chaste, Nyden, Wentz, Stahlberg, Herbrecht, Stopin, Anckarsater, Gillberg, Rastam and Leboyer2009). Both conditions can have a large impact on the daily life of affected people and their families, which is aggravated when both conditions co-occur (Anckarsater et al. Reference Anckarsater, Stahlberg, Larson, Hakansson, Jutblad, Niklasson, Nyden, Wentz, Westergren, Cloninger, Gillberg and Rastam2006). Yet little is known about the etiology of symptoms related to ADHD and ASD in adulthood and the pattern of co-occurrence.

The co-occurrence of psychiatric disorders may arise for several reasons (Neale & Kendler, Reference Neale and Kendler1995; Riso et al. Reference Riso, Klein, Ferro, Kasch, Pepper, Schwartz and Aronson1996; Cramer et al. Reference Cramer, Waldorp, van der Maas and Borsboom2010). For instance, the affection status of one disorder may increase the risk of developing a second disorder. Co-occurrence may also be due to chance, when two disorders are autonomous, independent disorders. Alternatively, co-occurrence may arise because of a shared etiological factor. Given the very early and often parallel onset of both ADHD and ASD, and the fact that co-morbidity is much higher than would be expected based on the prevalence of each disorder (i.e. 30–50% of adult patients with ASD show ADHD symptoms, and vice versa) (Stahlberg et al. Reference Stahlberg, Soderstrom, Rastam and Gillberg2004; Anckarsater et al. Reference Anckarsater, Stahlberg, Larson, Hakansson, Jutblad, Niklasson, Nyden, Wentz, Westergren, Cloninger, Gillberg and Rastam2006; Hofvander et al. Reference Hofvander, Delorme, Chaste, Nyden, Wentz, Stahlberg, Herbrecht, Stopin, Anckarsater, Gillberg, Rastam and Leboyer2009), we assume that a shared etiological factor is most likely.

Because of the early onset of both disorders, etiological research into ADHD and ASD has focused mainly on children, and most studies have been conducted in population-based twin and family samples. Population-based studies are, next to clinical studies, a useful design for examining disorder characteristics. The underlying symptoms related to ADHD and ASD show a dimensional distribution in the general population (Constantino & Todd, Reference Constantino and Todd2003; Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Derks, Hudziak, Verhulst, Posthuma and Boomsma2007), enabling the use of trait measures and as such providing more power for quantitative genetic analyses than case–control designs. Moreover, the etiology of the conditions has been shown to be similar in the extreme end and in the normal variation of both ASD (Robinson et al. Reference Robinson, Koenen, McCormick, Munir, Hallett, Happe, Plomin and Ronald2011; Lundstrom et al. Reference Lundstrom, Chang, Rastam, Gillberg, Larsson, Anckarsater and Lichtenstein2012) and ADHD (Larsson et al. Reference Larsson, Anckarsater, Rastam, Chang and Lichtenstein2012). Heritability estimates of around 75% for both traits suggest that genetic factors play an important role in the etiology of both ADHD- (Nikolas & Burt, Reference Nikolas and Burt2010) and ASD-related traits (Ronald & Hoekstra, Reference Ronald and Hoekstra2011) during childhood, and that environmental factors play a more modest role. Few studies have investigated the etiological factors of ADHD and ASD traits in adult samples. Heritability estimates of around 35% have been observed for ADHD symptoms (van den Berg et al. Reference van den Berg, Willemsen, de Geus and Boomsma2006; Boomsma et al. Reference Boomsma, Saviouk, Hottenga, Distel, de Moor, Vink, Geels, van Beek, Bartels, de Geus and Willemsen2010), and Hoekstra et al. (Reference Hoekstra, Bartels, Verweij and Boomsma2007) reported a heritability of 57% for autistic traits in a sample of 18-year-old twins. Thus, these studies suggest that genetic factors continue to play a role in ADHD- and ASD-related traits in adulthood, but to a smaller extent than during childhood (for a review on heritability change over time in ADHD, see Franke et al. Reference Franke, Faraone, Asherson, Buitelaar, Bau, Ramos-Quiroga, Mick, Grevet, Johansson, Haavik, Lesch, Cormand and Reif2012).

The shared etiology of ADHD and ASD traits in children was investigated in three (non-clinical) twin studies, showing an increasing genetic correlation (r g) between ADHD and ASD traits with age. A study in 312 2-year-old twin pairs in the USA reported a genetic correlation of 0.27 (Ronald et al. Reference Ronald, Edelson, Asherson and Saudino2010). In a much larger sample of 8- to 9-year-old twins (n=6771) in UK families, a genetic correlation of 0.55 was found (Ronald et al. Reference Ronald, Simonoff, Kuntsi, Asherson and Plomin2008). Lastly, Lichtenstein et al. (Reference Lichtenstein, Carlstrom, Rastam, Gillberg and Anckarsater2010) observed a genetic correlation of 0.87 in a very large sample (>10 000) of 9- to 12-year-old Swedish twins. These findings indicate that genetic factors, common to both traits, play an important role in the co-occurrence of ADHD and ASD traits in children. The increasing genetic correlation with age suggests that the genetic overlap may become stronger in more advanced stages of child development, although these findings should be interpreted with caution as they all derive from cross-sectional rather than longitudinal studies, using different samples and different instruments to assess ADHD and ASD traits.

Two twin studies have investigated the etiology of the co-occurrence of ADHD and autistic traits in adults. Reiersen et al. (Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008) measured autistic traits with 11 items of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and ADHD traits with 12 items of DSM-IV (n = 674 participants, mean age 23 years). The SRS and ADHD scores were significantly correlated on a phenotypic (r p = 0.48) and genetic level (r a = 0.72). More recently, Lundstrom et al. (Reference Lundstrom, Chang, Kerekes, Gumpert, Rastam, Gillberg, Lichtenstein and Anckarsater2011) investigated the phenotypic and genetic association between autistic traits and ADHD traits (both trait measures based on DSM-IV items) in a nationwide sample of 18 349 adults (age range 20–47 years). They observed a phenotypic correlation of 0.44 and a genetic correlation of 0.53.

In sum, research on ADHD and ASD in adults is very sparse, and therefore understanding the etiology of ASD and ADHD traits and their co-occurrence in adulthood is, as yet, poor. The current study aimed to fill this gap by investigating the co-occurrence of ADHD traits and autistic traits, and their shared etiology, in detail. Whereas previous studies focused on the association between simple composite measures of ADHD and ASD, we aimed to investigate this association in depth by focusing on dimensional measures of the components of ADHD and ASD. Such analyses could enhance our understanding of the co-morbidity patterns as seen in clinical practice, and help to identify shared genetic underpinnings.

To this end, we used five autistic traits that are related to known behavioral or cognitive characteristics of ASD: (1) social skills impairment, (2) a strong routine preference, (3) difficulties with attentional switching, (4) imagination impairments, and (5) a fascination for numbers and patterns. In addition, we distinguished traits of subtypes of ADHD as used in DSM-IV (i.e. Hyperactive/Impulsive and Inattentive subtype and Combined subtype) in one sample, and focused on Attention Problems (AP) in a second, independent sample. The aim of this study was twofold: first, to examine in detail the phenotypic association of autistic traits with ADHD traits and AP in two independent adult general population samples; and second, to investigate the genetic and environmental etiology of the co-occurrence of autistic traits and AP.

Method

Sample

Participants volunteered in the Netherlands Study of Cognition, Environment and Genes (NESCOG), which comprises a general population sample (sample 1) and a genetically informative, family-based sample (sample 2). The aim of NESCOG is to study individual differences in cognition and cognitive-related disorders. Sample 1 (S1, n = 837) was recruited through media advertisement or through the Science Live Program of the NEMO Science Center in Amsterdam (www.sciencelive.nl). Apart from cognitive tasks, participants were asked to complete an online behavioral questionnaire concerning life events, personality, environmental factors and behavioural conditions, which included ADHD traits and autistic traits. At the time of writing, the questionnaire had been completed by 559 subjects (67%, mean age 41.51 years, s.d. = 11.29, range 17–78 years), of whom 371 (66%) were females. Four parent–offspring and around 140 spousal relationships were present in this sample. Following categorization of educational attainment used previously in Dutch studies (van der Sluis et al. Reference van der Sluis, Willemsen, de Geus, Boomsma and Posthuma2008), the distribution of education was as follows: primary school only, 0.5%; lower vocational school and lower secondary school, 6.6%; intermediate vocational school and intermediate or higher secondary school, 32.9%; higher vocational school and university, 59.9%.

Sample 2 (S2, n = 560; 330 females) was a general population-based family sample that had participated in a study on the genetics of cognition and the interplay with life events (Vinkhuyzen et al. Reference Vinkhuyzen, van der Sluis, de Geus, Boomsma and Posthuma2010). The mean age in this sample was 46.58 years (s.d. = 12.40, range 23–75 years). All participants completed a behavioral questionnaire (slightly different from that completed by S1) about life events, environmental factors and behavioral conditions, including items on AP and autistic traits. We used data from twins [150 complete twin pairs, of which 82 were monozygotic (MZ), 68 dizygotic (DZ) and 87 incomplete twin pairs (28 MZ)], one triplet and all full siblings (n = 172). Zygosity of same-sex twin pairs was based on survey questions about physical similarity and confusion of twins by family members and strangers, or on DNA polymorphisms (97 pairs). For more details on S2, see Vinkhuyzen et al. (Reference Vinkhuyzen, van der Sluis, de Geus, Boomsma and Posthuma2010).

The Central Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects in The Netherlands provided institutional review board (IRB) approval for both NESCOG samples in this study. After complete description of the study to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Measures

ADHD traits

In S1, ADHD traits were measured with the self-reported Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). Thirty items were rated on a four-point scale (1 = never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, and 4 = very often) regarding behavior in the past months. Four subscales can be derived: Inattention (CAARS-I, nine items), Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (CAARS-HI, nine items), ADHD total (sum of Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity items) and ADHD index (CAARS index, 12 items). The ADHD index differs from the ADHD total in that it specifically identifies subjects who are likely to have an ADHD diagnosis according to DSM-IV criteria (Conners et al. Reference Conners, Erhardt and Sparrow1999). The CAARS-I and CAARS-HI scales were analysed to investigate the relationship between specific dimensions of ADHD traits and autistic traits. To examine the relationship between a potential ADHD diagnosis and autistic traits, we also incorporated the CAARS index scale.

AP

Participants in S2 completed the AP scale of the Young Adult Self-Report (YASR; Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1997). The YASR-AP scale consists of seven items that mainly focus on inattentive symptoms. It is rated on a three-point scale (1 = never, 2 = sometimes and 3 = often) regarding behavior in the past 6 months.

Autistic traits

In both samples, autistic traits were measured with the abridged version of the self-report Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ; Baron-Cohen et al. Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin and Clubley2001). The abridged version (AQ-Short) comprises 28 items rated on a four-point scale (1 = definitely agree, 2 = slightly agree, 3 = slightly disagree, and 4 = definitely disagree). If applicable, items are scored reversely such that a higher score refers to more autistic problems. High scores on the AQ-Short have been shown to be indicative for ASD in Dutch and in UK populations (Hoekstra et al. Reference Hoekstra, Vinkhuyzen, Wheelwright, Bartels, Boomsma, Baron-Cohen, Posthuma and van der Sluis2011). Along with a total score (AQ-Short Total), five subscales can be derived from the AQ-Short, namely Social Skills (AQ-Short Social; seven items), Routine (AQ-Short Routine; four items), Attentional Switching (AQ-Short Switch; four items), Imagination (AQ-Short Imagination; eight items), and Numbers and Patterns (AQ-Short Numbers; five items).

Test–retest reliability

Out of a small independent population sample, comprising 37 parent–offspring pairs, 60 participants completed the behavioral questionnaire of S2 twice in 2 months. These data were used to examine the test–retest reliability of the AQ-Short scales and the YASR-AP scale. Written consent was obtained from each participant.

Statistical analyses

To accommodate familial or spousal dependencies in S1, S2 and the test–retest sample, all analyses were conducted with structural equation modeling in Mx (Neale et al. Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2006). Mx provides parameter estimates by maximizing the raw data likelihood. Estimates of means, variances and phenotypic correlations were obtained from a saturated model in which the phenotypic variance was not decomposed into genetic or environmental factors. Additionally, in S2, correlations were estimated for each measure between twins (intra-pair MZ and DZ twin correlations) and between twins and siblings (twin–sibling correlations). By constraining DZ twin correlations to be equal to twin–sibling correlations, we could test whether they differed significantly. Within-subject correlations between phenotypes (r p) were constrained to be equal across zygosity and all family members. In both S1 and S2 we tested whether the covariates of sex and age had a significant effect on the means. The applied model was μ = α + β1sex + β2age, where β values were based on raw data, sex was coded 0 for males and 1 for females, and age was given as actual age in years.

Specific hypotheses (e.g. r p deviates significantly from zero) were evaluated using hierarchical likelihood ratio (χ2) tests. The χ2 statistic is computed by taking twice the difference between the log-likelihood of a reference model and the log-likelihood of a submodel with certain constraints, and the associated degrees of freedom are computed as the difference in degrees of freedom between the two models (Rijsdijk, Reference Rijsdijk, Ferreira, Neale, Medland and Posthuma2007).

Twin–sibling design

Information on MZ and DZ twins and non-twin siblings was used to decompose the observed variance of a particular trait, and also the covariance between traits, into latent genetic and environmental variance components. These components are additive genetic influences (A, additive effects of genes at multiple loci), dominant genetic influences (D, interaction of genetic effects at the same loci), environmental influences that are shared among family members (C, common, shared environmental effects), and environmental influences that are unique for a person (E). Because MZ twins are genetically identical, they share all their additive and dominant genetic effects. DZ twins and normal siblings share, on average, half of their segregating genes and therefore, on average, half of their additive genetic effects and a quarter of their dominant genetic effects. All twins and siblings in this sample grew up in the same family and thus share their family environment.

Genetic models

The choice of an AE, ACE or ADE model is usually based on the MZ correlations and DZ/twin–sibling correlations. MZ correlations twice as high as DZ/twin–sibling correlations indicate the presence of additive genetic influences (AE), whereas DZ/twin–sibling correlations lower than half the MZ correlations indicate the presence of dominant genetic influences (ADE; as dominant genetic effects can only exist in the presence of additive genetic effects, A and D are necessarily both included in the models). DZ/twin–sibling correlations higher than half the MZ correlations indicate potential shared environmental influences (ACE), and MZ correlations that are of similar magnitude to DZ/twin–sibling correlations indicate that only environmental influences play a role (CE). The unique environmental component (E) includes measurement error and is therefore always included in the models. Because common environmental effects (C) make family members more similar whereas dominant genetic effects (D) reduce the expected phenotypic resemblance in DZ twins and siblings relative to MZ twins, the effects of C and D cannot be estimated simultaneously in the twin model (Boomsma et al. Reference Boomsma, Busjahn and Peltonen2002).

The genetic or environmental variance explained is usually reported in a standardized form, as a proportion, by dividing this part of the variance by the total phenotypic variance. The proportion of variance that is explained by genetic effects (A and D) is called the broad-sense heritability estimate (i.e. heritability is calculated as genetic variance divided by the total phenotypic variance). As the power to detect sex differences on the variance and covariance components was expected to be low (Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Stins, Posthuma, Gosso, Verhulst and Boomsma2006), we combined male and female data. This choice seems justified as there is no evidence that sex moderates co-morbidity patterns in ADHD (Biederman et al. Reference Biederman, Faraone, Monuteaux, Bober and Cadogen2004), or that sex influences variance/covariance decomposition of ADHD and ASD traits in adults (Reiersen et al. Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008).

Results

In S2, two clear outliers (both males) with scores >3 standard deviations (s.d.) from the mean for the AQ-Short total scale and the AP scale were removed. Test–retest correlations were 0.68 for the AP scale, 0.83 for AQ-Short Total, 0.80 for AQ-Short Social, 0.65 for AQ-Short Routine, 0.66 for AQ-Short Switch, 0.75 for AQ-Short Imagination and 0.88 for AQ-Short Numbers. Test–retest measures were only conducted for the S2 questionnaire data. We have no data on the reliability of the CAARS measures in S1. However, a previous study reported a 6-month test–retest correlation of 0.67 for the CAARS index (Boomsma et al. Reference Boomsma, Saviouk, Hottenga, Distel, de Moor, Vink, Geels, van Beek, Bartels, de Geus and Willemsen2010). Values of Cronbach's α for the measured scales in S1 and S2 were acceptable (0.62–0.78 and 0.50–0.77 respectively), given the low number of items for some of the scales (Cortina, Reference Cortina1993). Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for all measures in S1 and S2, including significant sex and age effects on the mean. Significant effects only were included in the subsequent analyses. The means of S1 and S2 on the AQ scales were not significantly different, except for the AQ-Short Switch scale (fit statistics not shown).

Table 1. Estimated means, standard deviations (s.d.), significant sex and age effects for the AQ-Short scales, CAARS ADHD symptom scales (S1) and YASR Attention Problems (AP) scale (S2)

AQ-Short, Abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient; CAARS, Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scales; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; YASR, Young Adult Self-Report.

The applied model is μ = α + β1sex + β2age, where β values are based on raw data, sex is coded 0 for males and 1 for females, and age is given as actual age in years.

Table 2 presents phenotypic correlations of the CAARS scales and the AP scale with the AQ-Short scales. In S1, most correlations between CAARS-HI and the AQ-Short scales were non-significant or low, two correlations were significant for CAARS-I and almost all correlations were significant for the CAARS index scale. Most prominent was the correlation between the AQ-Short Switch scale and both the CAARS-I and CAARS index scale (r = 0.47 and 0.36 respectively). This finding was replicated in S2, where the AP scale correlated most prominently with the AQ-Short Switch scale (r = 0.32).

Table 2. Estimated phenotypic correlations of the AQ-Short scales with the CAARS ADHD symptom scales in S1 and the YASR Attention Problems (AP) scale in S2

AQ-Short, Abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient; CAARS, Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scales; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; YASR, Young Adult Self-Report.

a Significant at p < 0.01; all correlations were based on means corrected for sex and age effects where appropriate.

Genetic results

Table 3 gives the twin correlations and standardized estimates of additive and dominant genetic effects, and common and unique environmental effects, based on univariate models. DZ twin correlations were not significantly different from twin–sibling correlations on any of the scales. Overall, MZ twin correlations were higher than DZ/twin–sibling correlations indicating genetic influences. As the equated DZ/twin–sibling correlations were lower than half the MZ correlation, we allowed for dominant genetic effects by fitting an ADE model, except for AQ-Short Numbers, where an ACE model was applied. We tested whether C or D could be eliminated from the models (Table 4 a). The most parsimonious model for all scales was a model with A and E, apart from AQ-Short Routine. Broad heritability estimates ranged between 28% and 51%.

Table 3. Left: Monozygotic (MZ), dizygotic (DZ) and twin-sibling correlations in S2. Right: The contribution of additive genetic (A), dominant genetic (D), and common (C) and unique environmental (E) variance to the total variance of the AQ-Short scales and the YASR AP scale, based on univariate models

AQ-Short, Abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient; AP, Attention Problems; YASR, Young Adult Self-Report.

Estimates from univariate models; the models were based on means corrected for sex and age where appropriate. 95% Confidence intervals are given in parentheses.

Table 4. Model-fitting results of (a) univariate genetic models for the AQ-Short scales and the YASR AP scale and (b) bivariate genetic models for the AQ-Short Total, AQ-Short Routine and AQ-Short Switch scale with the YASR AP scale

AQ-Short, Abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient; YASR, Young Adult Self-Report; LL, log likelihood; df, degrees of freedom; CI, confidence interval; AP, Attention Problems.

a Compared to saturated model.

b Compared to ADE model.

For AQ-Short scales that were significantly correlated with the AP scale in S2, we investigated whether the covariance between scales was best explained by additive genetic or unique environmental factors (i.e. AQ-Short Total, Switch and Routine). MZ cross-trait/cross-twin correlations were higher than DZ/twin–sibling cross-trait correlations on all three scales, with respectively 0.20, 0.34 and 0.10 for MZ twins and 0.04, 0.12 and 0.01 for DZ/twin–siblings. Table 4 b shows the fit statistics of a series of bivariate genetic models that we subsequently tested for each AQ scale. Dropping D from the models gave no significant worsening of model fits. Subsequent tests showed that, for AP and AQ-Short Total, covariance due to genetic factors could not be eliminated whereas dropping the covariance due to unique environmental factors did not worsen the model fit. Genetic factors explained all of the phenotypic correlation between these scales. The genetic correlation of 0.40 suggested that partly overlapping genetic factors play a role in the AP score and the overall autistic traits score. For the AP and AQ-Short Routine scale, the pattern was less clear as either covariance due to genetic, or unique environmental factors could be deleted from the model, whereas deleting both was not allowed. Finally, for AP and the AQ-Short Switch scale, the covariance was explained fully by genetic factors. A genetic correlation of 0.80 suggested that a set of overlapping genes has an influence on the AP score and also on the AQ-Short Switch score (for path loadings, see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Final (AE) model of Attention Problems (AP) and the abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-Short) scales for one individual with unstandardized path loadings, genetic (r a) and unique environmental (r e) correlation, including confidence intervals. A, latent additive genetic factor; E, latent unique environmental factor. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path loading.

$${\rm Heritability}\;{\rm AP} \equals {{a_{\setnum{11}}^{\setnum{2}} } \over {a_{\setnum{11}}^{\setnum{2}} \plus e_{\setnum{11}}^{\setnum{2}} }}\comma$$
$${\rm Heritability}\;{\rm AQ{\mbox {-}}Short\ Scale} \equals {{a_{\setnum{21}}^{\setnum{2}} \plus a_{\setnum{22}}^{\setnum{2}} } \over {a_{\setnum{21}}^{\setnum{2}} \plus a_{\setnum{22}}^{\setnum{2}} \plus e_{\setnum{21}}^{\setnum{2}} \plus e_{\setnum{22}}^{\setnum{2}} }}\comma$$
$${\rm Genetic}\;{\rm correlation}\;{\rm between}\;{\rm AP}\;{\rm and}\;{\rm AQ{\mbox {-}}Short}\;{\rm Scale} \equals {{a_{\setnum{11}} \times a_{\setnum{21}} } \over {\sqrt {a_{\setnum{11}}^{\setnum{2}} } \times \sqrt {a_{\setnum{21}}^{\setnum{2}} \plus a_{\setnum{22}}^{\setnum{2}} } }}.$$

Discussion

This is the first study to examine the (genetic) relationship between specific characteristics of ADHD and autistic traits in adults. The strength of our study is that we report remarkably similar phenotypic findings in two independent samples with two different measures of ADHD traits/AP: first, an apparent association between inattentive traits of ADHD and the attentional switching dimension of autistic traits; and second, a low to moderate association with the remaining autistic trait dimensions. We strengthen our findings with genetic analyses in one of the samples (S2) with a twin–sibling design. Upon the clear phenotypic association, an important genetic overlap between attentional switching and AP was observed. Our findings fit well in the ‘fractionable triad hypothesis’ proposed by Happe & Ronald (Reference Happe and Ronald2008), which states that largely independent genes operate on different characteristics of autism. Following that reasoning we would expect that the shared genetic etiology between AP and autistic traits is most evident in specific autistic characteristics rather than the whole autistic spectrum. Indeed, our study suggests that the ability to switch attention specifically represents this shared genetic etiology.

Our results are not completely in line with the study by Reiersen et al. (Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008), who reported a phenotypic correlation of 0.47 and a genetic correlation of 0.72 between ADHD traits representing mainly inattention (which fits with our results) and autistic traits mainly referring to reciprocal social behavior and, to a lesser extent, repetitive behaviors. The latter deviates from our findings, as social skills and routine were only modestly associated with inattentive behavior in our samples. The sample in the study of Reiersen et al. (Reference Reiersen, Constantino, Grimmer, Martin and Todd2008) was comparable to those in the current study except that the age was much lower than in the current samples (mean age of 23 years v. 41/46 years); this might explain the difference in results.

In sum, this study suggests that the co-occurrence of ADHD symptoms and ASD traits is not driven by behavioral impairments such as hyperactivity, social skills, imagination or routine preferences but to shared problems on an attentional level. Our findings did not result from an overlap in AQ-Short Attentional Switching items and the Inattention items of the CAARS (S1, e.g. ‘I cannot concentrate when at work’, ‘I have trouble finishing tasks’) or the items of the AP scale (S2, e.g. ‘I daydream a lot’, ‘I cannot focus my attention’). The AQ-Short Attentional Switching scale refers specifically to ‘the ability to easily switch attention’, ‘perform simultaneously multiple tasks’ or ‘follow multiple conversations’, daily skills that need concentration and cognitive flexibility. Recently, Kessler et al. (Reference Kessler, Green, Adler, Barkley, Chatterji, Faraone, Finkelman, Greenhill, Gruber, Jewell, Russo, Sampson and Van Brunt2010) showed that similar deficits in executive functioning (defined by the authors as daily life self-regulation, such as ‘the ability to organize, prioritize and integrate cognitive functions’) were the most discriminating predictors of adult ADHD. They suggested that a working memory deficit could underlie these symptoms. Attentional switching and working memory largely depend on prefrontal processing (Diamond, Reference Diamond2011), which in turn is modulated by dopaminergic systems (Cools & D'Esposito, Reference Cools and D'Esposito2011), which have previously been associated with ADHD (Franke et al. Reference Franke, Faraone, Asherson, Buitelaar, Bau, Ramos-Quiroga, Mick, Grevet, Johansson, Haavik, Lesch, Cormand and Reif2012). Executive functioning deficits, related to cognitive flexibility, have been reported for ASD and ADHD in children; however, findings were not always consistent (for an overview, see Rommelse et al. Reference Rommelse, Geurts, Franke, Buitelaar and Hartman2011).

A general finding from recent genetic studies of complex traits such as ASD and ADHD is polygenicity (Visscher et al. Reference Visscher, Brown, McCarthy and Yang2012). Common types of complex traits are associated with genetic variation at hundreds of loci. Therefore, it would not be expected that a few genes account for the variation in ADHD and ASD, or for the co-morbidity between them. Recent genome-wide association studies (GWAS) on autism (Anney et al. Reference Anney, Klei, Pinto, Regan, Conroy, Magalhaes, Correia, Abrahams, Sykes, Pagnamenta, Almeida, Bacchelli, Bailey, Baird, Battaglia, Berney, Bolshakova, Bolte, Bolton, Bourgeron, Brennan, Brian, Carson, Casallo, Casey, Chu, Cochrane, Corsello, Crawford, Crossett, Dawson, de Jonge, Delorme, Drmic, Duketis, Duque, Estes, Farrar, Fernandez, Folstein, Fombonne, Freitag, Gilbert, Gillberg, Glessner, Goldberg, Green, Guter, Hakonarson, Heron, Hill, Holt, Howe, Hughes, Hus, Igliozzi, Kim, Klauck, Kolevzon, Korvatska, Kustanovich, Lajonchere, Lamb, Laskawiec, Leboyer, Le Couteur, Leventhal, Lionel, Liu, Lord, Lotspeich, Lund, Maestrini, Mahoney, Mantoulan, Marshall, McConachie, McDougle, McGrath, McMahon, Melhem, Merikangas, Migita, Minshew, Mirza, Munson, Nelson, Noakes, Noor, Nygren, Oliveira, Papanikolaou, Parr, Parrini, Paton, Pickles, Piven, Posey, Poustka, Poustka, Prasad, Ragoussis, Renshaw, Rickaby, Roberts, Roeder, Roge, Rutter, Bierut, Rice, Salt, Sansom, Sato, Segurado, Senman, Shah, Sheffield, Soorya, Sousa, Stoppioni, Strawbridge, Tancredi, Tansey, Thiruvahindrapduram, Thompson, Thomson, Tryfon, Tsiantis, Van Engeland, Vincent, Volkmar, Wallace, Wang, Wang, Wassink, Wing, Wittemeyer, Wood, Yaspan, Zurawiecki, Zwaigenbaum, Betancur, Buxbaum, Cantor, Cook, Coon, Cuccaro, Gallagher, Geschwind, Gill, Haines, Miller, Monaco, Nurnberger, Paterson, Pericak-Vance, Schellenberg, Scherer, Sutcliffe, Szatmari, Vicente, Vieland, Wijsman, Devlin, Ennis and Hallmayer2010) and ADHD (Neale et al. Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Anney, Asherson, Buitelaar, Franke, Gill, Kent, Holmans, Middleton, Thapar, Lesch, Faraone, Daly, Nguyen, Schafer, Steinhausen, Reif, Renner, Romanos, Romanos, Warnke, Walitza, Freitag, Meyer, Palmason, Rothenberger, Hawi, Sergeant, Roeyers, Mick and Biederman2010) have not revealed any genome-wide significant hits, probably because the sample sizes were too small. An increasing number of clinical studies have reported on rare variants associated with ADHD (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Zaharieva, Martin, Langley, Mantripragada, Fossdal, Stefansson, Stefansson, Magnusson, Gudmundsson, Gustafsson, Holmans, Owen, O'Donovan and Thapar2010; Elia et al. Reference Elia, Gai, Xie, Perin, Geiger, Glessner, D'Arcy, deBerardinis, Frackelton, Kim, Lantieri, Muganga, Wang, Takeda, Rappaport, Grant, Berrettini, Devoto, Shaikh, Hakonarson and White2011) and ASD (Pinto et al. Reference Pinto, Pagnamenta, Klei, Anney, Merico, Regan, Conroy, Magalhaes, Correia, Abrahams, Almeida, Bacchelli, Bader, Bailey, Baird, Battaglia, Berney, Bolshakova, Bolte, Bolton, Bourgeron, Brennan, Brian, Bryson, Carson, Casallo, Casey, Chung, Cochrane, Corsello, Crawford, Crossett, Cytrynbaum, Dawson, de Jonge, Delorme, Drmic, Duketis, Duque, Estes, Farrar, Fernandez, Folstein, Fombonne, Freitag, Gilbert, Gillberg, Glessner, Goldberg, Green, Green, Guter, Hakonarson, Heron, Hill, Holt, Howe, Hughes, Hus, Igliozzi, Kim, Klauck, Kolevzon, Korvatska, Kustanovich, Lajonchere, Lamb, Laskawiec, Leboyer, Le Couteur, Leventhal, Lionel, Liu, Lord, Lotspeich, Lund, Maestrini, Mahoney, Mantoulan, Marshall, McConachie, McDougle, McGrath, McMahon, Merikangas, Migita, Minshew, Mirza, Munson, Nelson, Noakes, Noor, Nygren, Oliveira, Papanikolaou, Parr, Parrini, Paton, Pickles, Pilorge, Piven, Ponting, Posey, Poustka, Poustka, Prasad, Ragoussis, Renshaw, Rickaby, Roberts, Roeder, Roge, Rutter, Bierut, Rice, Salt, Sansom, Sato, Segurado, Sequeira, Senman, Shah, Sheffield, Soorya, Sousa, Stein, Sykes, Stoppioni, Strawbridge, Tancredi, Tansey, Thiruvahindrapduram, Thompson, Thomson, Tryfon, Tsiantis, Van Engeland, Vincent, Volkmar, Wallace, Wang, Wang, Wassink, Webber, Weksberg, Wing, Wittemeyer, Wood, Wu, Yaspan, Zurawiecki, Zwaigenbaum, Buxbaum, Cantor, Cook, Coon, Cuccaro, Devlin, Ennis, Gallagher, Geschwind, Gill, Haines, Hallmayer, Miller, Monaco, Nurnberger, Paterson, Pericak-Vance, Schellenberg, Szatmari, Vicente, Vieland, Wijsman, Scherer, Sutcliffe and Betancur2010; Levy et al. Reference Levy, Ronemus, Yamrom, Lee, Leotta, Kendall, Marks, Lakshmi, Pai, Ye, Buja, Krieger, Yoon, Troge, Rodgers, Iossifov and Wigler2011), of which some were overlapping (Taurines et al. Reference Taurines, Schwenck, Westerwald, Sachse, Siniatchkin and Freitag2012; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Franke, Mick, Anney, Freitag, Gill, Thapar, O'Donovan, Owen, Holmans, Kent, Middleton, Zhang-James, Liu, Meyer, Nguyen, Romanos, Romanos, Seitz, Renner, Walitza, Warnke, Palmason, Buitelaar, Rommelse, Vasquez, Hawi, Langley, Sergeant, Steinhausen, Roeyers, Biederman, Zaharieva, Hakonarson, Elia, Lionel, Crosbie, Marshall, Schachar, Scherer, Todorov, Smalley, Loo, Nelson, Shtir, Asherson, Reif, Lesch and Faraone2012). However, it is difficult to interpret the functional implications of these results. Many identified mutations are in non-coding regions of the genome, others are involved in a variety of cellular processes (e.g. signaling pathways, protein interaction networks), and no clear pattern can yet be discerned. Our results suggest that biological pathways related to attentional control might be involved in specific co-morbidity patterns of ADHD and ASD in adults. Hence, attentional control might be a useful (endo)phenotype in future genetic studies.

The following methodological considerations warrant discussion. First, our measures were based only on self-reports. With multiple informants, situational variation in behavior can be taken into account, and in twin analyses potential sibling interaction issues can be examined. It has also been suggested that self-ratings produce lower heritability estimates (see Franke et al. Reference Franke, Faraone, Asherson, Buitelaar, Bau, Ramos-Quiroga, Mick, Grevet, Johansson, Haavik, Lesch, Cormand and Reif2012); indeed, the heritability estimates as presented in the current study are lower than those in children, where usually parental or teacher ratings are used. Second, the family sample was underpowered to detect genetic dominance effects (Posthuma & Boomsma, Reference Posthuma and Boomsma2000). However, using a very large sample, Boomsma et al. (Reference Boomsma, Saviouk, Hottenga, Distel, de Moor, Vink, Geels, van Beek, Bartels, de Geus and Willemsen2010) showed recently that ADHD trait variance is best explained with the model we applied to our data. Third, it is important to note that this study was performed in non-clinical population-based samples; the picture may be different in clinical populations. The ADHD index scale used in S1 is indicative of an ADHD diagnosis, and indeed showed somewhat higher correlations with the AQ-Short scales compared to the other scales. It would be of interest to examine the association between ADHD symptoms and similar autism measures in clinical (preferably family) samples.

To summarize, our results suggest that ADHD traits and autistic traits in adults are associated as a result of attention-related problems. As the nature of this association is purely genetic, biological pathways involving attentional control might be the focus of future studies aimed at finding genes for ASD and ADHD.

Acknowledgments

We thank all participating subjects. This research was part of Science Live, the innovative research program of science center NEMO that enables scientists to carry out peer-reviewed research using NEMO visitors as volunteers. Collaboration between P.F.S. and the CNCR was facilitated by the Royal Academy of Arts and Sciences of The Netherlands, Visiting Professor Program (ISK/5913/VPP). The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) Division for the Social Sciences (MaGW) provided funding for this research through grant VENI-451-08-025 to S.vdS. and VIDI 016-065-318 to D.P.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Achenbach, TM (1997). Manual for the Young Adult Self-Report and Young Adult Behavior Checklist. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont: Burlington, VT.Google Scholar
Anckarsater, H, Stahlberg, O, Larson, T, Hakansson, C, Jutblad, SB, Niklasson, L, Nyden, A, Wentz, E, Westergren, S, Cloninger, CR, Gillberg, C, Rastam, M (2006). The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 12391244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anney, R, Klei, L, Pinto, D, Regan, R, Conroy, J, Magalhaes, TR, Correia, C, Abrahams, BS, Sykes, N, Pagnamenta, AT, Almeida, J, Bacchelli, E, Bailey, AJ, Baird, G, Battaglia, A, Berney, T, Bolshakova, N, Bolte, S, Bolton, PF, Bourgeron, T, Brennan, S, Brian, J, Carson, AR, Casallo, G, Casey, J, Chu, SH, Cochrane, L, Corsello, C, Crawford, EL, Crossett, A, Dawson, G, de Jonge, M, Delorme, R, Drmic, I, Duketis, E, Duque, F, Estes, A, Farrar, P, Fernandez, BA, Folstein, SE, Fombonne, E, Freitag, CM, Gilbert, J, Gillberg, C, Glessner, JT, Goldberg, J, Green, J, Guter, SJ, Hakonarson, H, Heron, EA, Hill, M, Holt, R, Howe, JL, Hughes, G, Hus, V, Igliozzi, R, Kim, C, Klauck, SM, Kolevzon, A, Korvatska, O, Kustanovich, V, Lajonchere, CM, Lamb, JA, Laskawiec, M, Leboyer, M, Le Couteur, A, Leventhal, BL, Lionel, AC, Liu, XQ, Lord, C, Lotspeich, L, Lund, SC, Maestrini, E, Mahoney, W, Mantoulan, C, Marshall, CR, McConachie, H, McDougle, CJ, McGrath, J, McMahon, WM, Melhem, NM, Merikangas, A, Migita, O, Minshew, NJ, Mirza, GK, Munson, J, Nelson, SF, Noakes, C, Noor, A, Nygren, G, Oliveira, G, Papanikolaou, K, Parr, JR, Parrini, B, Paton, T, Pickles, A, Piven, J, Posey, DJ, Poustka, A, Poustka, F, Prasad, A, Ragoussis, J, Renshaw, K, Rickaby, J, Roberts, W, Roeder, K, Roge, B, Rutter, ML, Bierut, LJ, Rice, JP, Salt, J, Sansom, K, Sato, D, Segurado, R, Senman, L, Shah, N, Sheffield, VC, Soorya, L, Sousa, I, Stoppioni, V, Strawbridge, C, Tancredi, R, Tansey, K, Thiruvahindrapduram, B, Thompson, AP, Thomson, S, Tryfon, A, Tsiantis, J, Van Engeland, H, Vincent, JB, Volkmar, F, Wallace, S, Wang, K, Wang, Z, Wassink, TH, Wing, K, Wittemeyer, K, Wood, S, Yaspan, BL, Zurawiecki, D, Zwaigenbaum, L, Betancur, C, Buxbaum, JD, Cantor, RM, Cook, EH, Coon, H, Cuccaro, ML, Gallagher, L, Geschwind, DH, Gill, M, Haines, JL, Miller, J, Monaco, AP, Nurnberger, JI Jr., Paterson, AD, Pericak-Vance, MA, Schellenberg, GD, Scherer, SW, Sutcliffe, JS, Szatmari, P, Vicente, AM, Vieland, VJ, Wijsman, EM, Devlin, B, Ennis, S, Hallmayer, J (2010). A genome-wide scan for common alleles affecting risk for autism. Human Molecular Genetics 19, 40724082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Skinner, R, Martin, J, Clubley, E (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31, 517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J, Faraone, SV, Monuteaux, MC, Bober, M, Cadogen, E (2004). Gender effects on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, revisited. Biological Psychiatry 55, 692700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boomsma, D, Busjahn, A, Peltonen, L (2002). Classical twin studies and beyond. Nature Review Genetics 3, 872882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boomsma, DI, Saviouk, V, Hottenga, JJ, Distel, MA, de Moor, MH, Vink, JM, Geels, LM, van Beek, JH, Bartels, M, de Geus, EJ, Willemsen, G (2010). Genetic epidemiology of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD index) in adults. PLoS ONE 5, e10621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugha, TS, McManus, S, Bankart, J, Scott, F, Purdon, S, Smith, J, Bebbington, P, Jenkins, R, Meltzer, H (2011). Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community in England. Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 459465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conners, CK, Erhardt, D, Sparrow, E (1999). Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scales. Multi-Health Systems: North Tonawanda, NY.Google Scholar
Constantino, JN, Todd, RD (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 524530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cools, R, D'Esposito, M (2011). Inverted U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biological Psychiatry 69, e113e125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cortina, JM (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology 78, 98104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cramer, AO, Waldorp, LJ, van der Maas, HL, Borsboom, D (2010). Comorbidity: a network perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 137150; discussion 150–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A (2011). Biological and social influences on cognitive control processes dependent on prefrontal cortex. Progress in Brain Research 189, 319339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elia, J, Gai, X, Xie, HM, Perin, JC, Geiger, E, Glessner, JT, D'Arcy, M, deBerardinis, R, Frackelton, E, Kim, C, Lantieri, F, Muganga, BM, Wang, L, Takeda, T, Rappaport, EF, Grant, SF, Berrettini, W, Devoto, M, Shaikh, TH, Hakonarson, H, White, PS (2011). Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Molecular Psychiatry 15, 637646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franke, B, Faraone, SV, Asherson, P, Buitelaar, J, Bau, CH, Ramos-Quiroga, JA, Mick, E, Grevet, EH, Johansson, S, Haavik, J, Lesch, KP, Cormand, B, Reif, A (2012). The genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, a review. Molecular Psychiatry 17, 960987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happe, F, Ronald, A (2008). The ‘fractionable autism triad’: a review of evidence from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychology Review 18, 287304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoekstra, RA, Bartels, M, Verweij, CJ, Boomsma, DI (2007). Heritability of autistic traits in the general population. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine 161, 372377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoekstra, RA, Vinkhuyzen, AA, Wheelwright, S, Bartels, M, Boomsma, DI, Baron-Cohen, S, Posthuma, D, van der Sluis, S (2011). The construction and validation of an abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-Short). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 41, 589596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofvander, B, Delorme, R, Chaste, P, Nyden, A, Wentz, E, Stahlberg, O, Herbrecht, E, Stopin, A, Anckarsater, H, Gillberg, C, Rastam, M, Leboyer, M (2009). Psychiatric and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry 9, 35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Green, JG, Adler, LA, Barkley, RA, Chatterji, S, Faraone, SV, Finkelman, M, Greenhill, LL, Gruber, MJ, Jewell, M, Russo, LJ, Sampson, NA, Van Brunt, DL (2010). Structure and diagnosis of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: analysis of expanded symptom criteria from the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 11681178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H, Anckarsater, H, Rastam, M, Chang, Z, Lichtenstein, P (2012). Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 53, 7380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, D, Ronemus, M, Yamrom, B, Lee, YH, Leotta, A, Kendall, J, Marks, S, Lakshmi, B, Pai, D, Ye, K, Buja, A, Krieger, A, Yoon, S, Troge, J, Rodgers, L, Iossifov, I, Wigler, M (2011). Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron 70, 886897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P, Carlstrom, E, Rastam, M, Gillberg, C, Anckarsater, H (2010). The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 13571363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundstrom, S, Chang, Z, Kerekes, N, Gumpert, CH, Rastam, M, Gillberg, C, Lichtenstein, P, Anckarsater, H (2011). Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults. Psychological Medicine 41, 24232433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundstrom, S, Chang, Z, Rastam, M, Gillberg, C, Larsson, H, Anckarsater, H, Lichtenstein, P (2012). Autism spectrum disorders and autistic-like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 4652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, BM, Medland, S, Ripke, S, Anney, RJ, Asherson, P, Buitelaar, J, Franke, B, Gill, M, Kent, L, Holmans, P, Middleton, F, Thapar, A, Lesch, KP, Faraone, SV, Daly, M, Nguyen, TT, Schafer, H, Steinhausen, HC, Reif, A, Renner, TJ, Romanos, M, Romanos, J, Warnke, A, Walitza, S, Freitag, C, Meyer, J, Palmason, H, Rothenberger, A, Hawi, Z, Sergeant, J, Roeyers, H, Mick, E, Biederman, J (2010). Case-control genome-wide association study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 906920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, MC, Boker, SM, Xie, G, Maes, HHM (2006). Mx: Statistical Modeling. Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University: Richmond, VA.Google Scholar
Neale, MC, Kendler, KS (1995). Models of comorbidity for multifactorial disorders. American Journal of Human Genetics 57, 935953.Google ScholarPubMed
Nikolas, MA, Burt, SA (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 119, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinto, D, Pagnamenta, AT, Klei, L, Anney, R, Merico, D, Regan, R, Conroy, J, Magalhaes, TR, Correia, C, Abrahams, BS, Almeida, J, Bacchelli, E, Bader, GD, Bailey, AJ, Baird, G, Battaglia, A, Berney, T, Bolshakova, N, Bolte, S, Bolton, PF, Bourgeron, T, Brennan, S, Brian, J, Bryson, SE, Carson, AR, Casallo, G, Casey, J, Chung, BH, Cochrane, L, Corsello, C, Crawford, EL, Crossett, A, Cytrynbaum, C, Dawson, G, de Jonge, M, Delorme, R, Drmic, I, Duketis, E, Duque, F, Estes, A, Farrar, P, Fernandez, BA, Folstein, SE, Fombonne, E, Freitag, CM, Gilbert, J, Gillberg, C, Glessner, JT, Goldberg, J, Green, A, Green, J, Guter, SJ, Hakonarson, H, Heron, EA, Hill, M, Holt, R, Howe, JL, Hughes, G, Hus, V, Igliozzi, R, Kim, C, Klauck, SM, Kolevzon, A, Korvatska, O, Kustanovich, V, Lajonchere, CM, Lamb, JA, Laskawiec, M, Leboyer, M, Le Couteur, A, Leventhal, BL, Lionel, AC, Liu, XQ, Lord, C, Lotspeich, L, Lund, SC, Maestrini, E, Mahoney, W, Mantoulan, C, Marshall, CR, McConachie, H, McDougle, CJ, McGrath, J, McMahon, WM, Merikangas, A, Migita, O, Minshew, NJ, Mirza, GK, Munson, J, Nelson, SF, Noakes, C, Noor, A, Nygren, G, Oliveira, G, Papanikolaou, K, Parr, JR, Parrini, B, Paton, T, Pickles, A, Pilorge, M, Piven, J, Ponting, CP, Posey, DJ, Poustka, A, Poustka, F, Prasad, A, Ragoussis, J, Renshaw, K, Rickaby, J, Roberts, W, Roeder, K, Roge, B, Rutter, ML, Bierut, LJ, Rice, JP, Salt, J, Sansom, K, Sato, D, Segurado, R, Sequeira, AF, Senman, L, Shah, N, Sheffield, VC, Soorya, L, Sousa, I, Stein, O, Sykes, N, Stoppioni, V, Strawbridge, C, Tancredi, R, Tansey, K, Thiruvahindrapduram, B, Thompson, AP, Thomson, S, Tryfon, A, Tsiantis, J, Van Engeland, H, Vincent, JB, Volkmar, F, Wallace, S, Wang, K, Wang, Z, Wassink, TH, Webber, C, Weksberg, R, Wing, K, Wittemeyer, K, Wood, S, Wu, J, Yaspan, BL, Zurawiecki, D, Zwaigenbaum, L, Buxbaum, JD, Cantor, RM, Cook, EH, Coon, H, Cuccaro, ML, Devlin, B, Ennis, S, Gallagher, L, Geschwind, DH, Gill, M, Haines, JL, Hallmayer, J, Miller, J, Monaco, AP, Nurnberger, JI Jr., Paterson, AD, Pericak-Vance, MA, Schellenberg, GD, Szatmari, P, Vicente, AM, Vieland, VJ, Wijsman, EM, Scherer, SW, Sutcliffe, JS, Betancur, C (2010). Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature 466, 368372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polderman, TJ, Derks, EM, Hudziak, JJ, Verhulst, FC, Posthuma, D, Boomsma, DI (2007). Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48, 10801087.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polderman, TJC, Stins, JF, Posthuma, D, Gosso, MF, Verhulst, FC, Boomsma, DI (2006). The phenotypic and genotypic relation between working memory speed and capacity. Intelligence 34, 549560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posthuma, D, Boomsma, DI (2000). A note on the statistical power in extended twin designs. Behavior Genetics 30, 147158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiersen, AM, Constantino, JN, Grimmer, M, Martin, NG, Todd, RD (2008). Evidence for shared genetic influences on self-reported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics 11, 579585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, F (2007). Introduction to statistics. In Statistical Genetics: Gene Mapping Through Linkage and Association (ed. Ferreira, M., Neale, B., Medland, S. and Posthuma, D.), pp. 4359. Taylor & Francis: London.Google Scholar
Riso, LP, Klein, DN, Ferro, T, Kasch, KL, Pepper, CM, Schwartz, JE, Aronson, TA (1996). Understanding the comorbidity between early-onset dysthymia and cluster B personality disorders: a family study. American Journal of Psychiatry 153, 900906.Google ScholarPubMed
Robinson, EB, Koenen, KC, McCormick, MC, Munir, K, Hallett, V, Happe, F, Plomin, R, Ronald, A (2011). Evidence that autistic traits show the same etiology in the general population and at the quantitative extremes (5%, 2.5%, and 1%). Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 11131121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rommelse, NN, Geurts, HM, Franke, B, Buitelaar, JK, Hartman, CA (2011). A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35, 13631396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A, Edelson, LR, Asherson, P, Saudino, KJ (2010). Exploring the relationship between autistic-like traits and ADHD behaviors in early childhood: findings from a community twin study of 2-year-olds. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 38, 185196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A, Hoekstra, RA (2011). Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics 156B, 255274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A, Simonoff, E, Kuntsi, J, Asherson, P, Plomin, R (2008). Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 535542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, V, Czobor, P, Balint, S, Meszaros, A, Bitter, I (2009). Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 194, 204211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahlberg, O, Soderstrom, H, Rastam, M, Gillberg, C (2004). Bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and other psychotic disorders in adults with childhood onset AD/HD and/or autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neural Transmission 111, 891902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taurines, R, Schwenck, C, Westerwald, E, Sachse, M, Siniatchkin, M, Freitag, C (2012). ADHD and autism: differential diagnosis or overlapping traits? A selective review. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 4, 115139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Berg, SM, Willemsen, G, de Geus, EJ, Boomsma, DI (2006). Genetic etiology of stability of attention problems in young adulthood. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics 141B, 5560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Sluis, S, Willemsen, G, de Geus, EJ, Boomsma, DI, Posthuma, D (2008). Gene-environment interaction in adults' IQ scores: measures of past and present environment. Behavior Genetics 38, 348360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinkhuyzen, AAE, van der Sluis, S, de Geus, EJC, Boomsma, DI, Posthuma, D (2010). Genetic influences on ‘environmental’ factors. Genes, Brain and Behavior 9, 276287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visscher, PM, Brown, MA, McCarthy, MI, Yang, J (2012). Five years of GWAS discovery. American Journal of Human Genetics 90, 724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, NM, Franke, B, Mick, E, Anney, RJ, Freitag, CM, Gill, M, Thapar, A, O'Donovan, MC, Owen, MJ, Holmans, P, Kent, L, Middleton, F, Zhang-James, Y, Liu, L, Meyer, J, Nguyen, TT, Romanos, J, Romanos, M, Seitz, C, Renner, TJ, Walitza, S, Warnke, A, Palmason, H, Buitelaar, J, Rommelse, N, Vasquez, AA, Hawi, Z, Langley, K, Sergeant, J, Steinhausen, HC, Roeyers, H, Biederman, J, Zaharieva, I, Hakonarson, H, Elia, J, Lionel, AC, Crosbie, J, Marshall, CR, Schachar, R, Scherer, SW, Todorov, A, Smalley, SL, Loo, S, Nelson, S, Shtir, C, Asherson, P, Reif, A, Lesch, KP, Faraone, SV (2012). Genome-wide analysis of copy number variants in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the role of rare variants and duplications at 15q13.3. American Journal of Psychiatry 169, 195204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, NM, Zaharieva, I, Martin, A, Langley, K, Mantripragada, K, Fossdal, R, Stefansson, H, Stefansson, K, Magnusson, P, Gudmundsson, OO, Gustafsson, O, Holmans, P, Owen, MJ, O'Donovan, M, Thapar, A (2010). Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 376, 14011408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Estimated means, standard deviations (s.d.), significant sex and age effects for the AQ-Short scales, CAARS ADHD symptom scales (S1) and YASR Attention Problems (AP) scale (S2)

Figure 1

Table 2. Estimated phenotypic correlations of the AQ-Short scales with the CAARS ADHD symptom scales in S1 and the YASR Attention Problems (AP) scale in S2

Figure 2

Table 3. Left: Monozygotic (MZ), dizygotic (DZ) and twin-sibling correlations in S2. Right: The contribution of additive genetic (A), dominant genetic (D), and common (C) and unique environmental (E) variance to the total variance of the AQ-Short scales and the YASR AP scale, based on univariate models

Figure 3

Table 4. Model-fitting results of (a) univariate genetic models for the AQ-Short scales and the YASR AP scale and (b) bivariate genetic models for the AQ-Short Total, AQ-Short Routine and AQ-Short Switch scale with the YASR AP scale

Figure 4

a

Figure 5

Fig. 1. Final (AE) model of Attention Problems (AP) and the abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-Short) scales for one individual with unstandardized path loadings, genetic (ra) and unique environmental (re) correlation, including confidence intervals. A, latent additive genetic factor; E, latent unique environmental factor. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path loading.