Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T15:43:48.327Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What makes a narrative memorable?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Allyssa Mccabe*
Affiliation:
Harvard University
Carole Peterson
Affiliation:
Memorial University, St. John's, Newfoundland
*
Address for Correspondence: Harvard University, Graduate School of Education, Larsen 711, Cambridge, MA 02138

Abstract

The relatively unexamined question of what kinds of narratives are memorable as whole units of discourse was addressed by studying memory for 288 children's personal narratives. Each narrative was assigned a structural category in story grammar analysis and Labov's highpoint analysis. Narratives were also rated for the presence or absence of sensational content. An incidental free recall of these narratives was then requested from 116 subjects. As predicted, an analysis of variance of recall frequencies revealed a main effect of sensational content; sensational narratives were recalled more frequently than nonsensational ones. Although there was no main effect of highpoint structure, that variable did interact with sensational content; in the absence of sensational content, highpoint structural sophistication predicted enhanced recall. Contrary to prediction, there were no main effects of highpoint structure or story grammar. The study is interpreted as providing partial validation of highpoint analysis, as well as revealing the independent, dominant contribution of content to memory for others' narratives as whole units.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Glenn, C. G. (1978). The role of episodic structure and of story length on children's recall of single stories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 229247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. A., & Hagreen, M. (1987, October). From script to story: The development of narrative skill. Paper presented at the 12th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Hudson, J. A., & Shapiro, L. R. (in press). Children's scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W., Cohen, P., Robins, C., & Lewis, J. (1968). A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican speakers in New York City: Vol. 2. (Cooperative Research Project No. 3288.) Washington, DC: Office of Education.Google Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 1244). Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1978). A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval. Discourse Processes, 1, 1435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & DeForest, M. (1979). Is there more than one way to recall a story? Child Development, 50, 886889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., Scribner, S., Cole, M., & DeForest, M. (1980). Cross-cultural invariance in story recall. Child Development, 51, 1926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1984). What makes a good story? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13, 457480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menig-Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (19771978). Children talk about death. Omega-Journal of Death and Dying, 8, 305317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, J. M., & Liebert, R. M. (1980). Science and behavior, 2nd ed. (pp. 3637). NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polanyi, L. (1985). Conversational story telling. In van Dijk, T. A. (Ed.), Handbook of discourse analysis (Vol. 3, pp. 183201). London: Academic.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In Bobrow, D. G. & Collins, A. (Eds.), Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In LaBerge, D. & Samuels, S. J. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1979). Topic selection in parent-child discourse. Discourse Processes, 2, 145153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SPSS, Inc. (1988). SPSS-X user's guide (3rd ed.). Chicago, IL: Author.Google Scholar
Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stein, N., & Nezworski, T. (1978). The effects of organization and instructional set on story memory. Discourse Processes, 1, 177193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 77110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, K. (1984). Ontological puzzles about narrative. Poetics, 13, 239259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar