Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T04:15:12.029Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Tailored nutrition education: is it really effective?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 October 2011

Helen Eyles*
Affiliation:
Clinical Trials Research Unit, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New Zealand
Cliona Ni Mhurchu
Affiliation:
Clinical Trials Research Unit, School of Population Health, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New Zealand
*
*Corresponding author: Email h.eyles@ctru.auckland.ac.nz
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

There has been a growing interest in tailored nutrition education over the previous decade, with a number of literature reviews suggesting this intervention strategy holds considerable potential. Nevertheless, the majority of intervention trials undertaken to date have employed subjective self-report outcome measures (such as dietary recalls). The aim of the present review is to further consider the likely true effect of tailored nutrition education by assessing the findings of tailored nutrition education intervention trials where objective outcome measures (such as sales data) have been employed.

Results

Four trials of tailored nutrition education employing objective outcome measures were identified: one was undertaken in eight low-cost supermarkets in New Zealand (2010; n 1104); one was an online intervention trial in Australia (2006; n 497); and two were undertaken in US supermarkets (1997 and 2001; n 105 and 296, respectively). Findings from the high-quality New Zealand trial were negative. Findings from the US trials were also generally negative, although reporting was poor making it difficult to assess quality. Findings from the high-quality online trial were positive, although have limited generalisability for public health.

Conclusions

Trials employing objective outcome measures strongly suggest tailored nutrition education is not effective as a stand-alone strategy. However, further large, high-quality trials employing objective outcome measures are needed to determine the true effectiveness of this popular nutrition intervention strategy. Regardless, education plays an important role in generating social understanding and acceptance of broader interventions to improve nutrition.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011

Tailored nutrition education is an individual-level intervention defined by Kreuter et al.(Reference Kreuter, Strecher and Glassman1) as ‘…any combination of information or change strategies intended to reach one specific person, based on characteristics that are unique to that person, are related to the outcome of interest, and have been derived from individual assessment’. Over the previous decade, there has been a growing interest in tailored nutrition education, particularly as developments in technology have enabled efficient personalisation and delivery of information to large numbers of individuals. Since 2006, four systematic literature reviews have suggested tailored nutrition education is effective in improving the dietary habits of adults(Reference Eyles and Ni Mhurchu2Reference Enwald and Huotari5). However, the majority of included trials have used subjective self-report outcome measures such as FFQ and dietary recalls. More objective measures of diet include sales receipts and electronic shopping data; although such data have some limitations (e.g. they may reflect food purchases of a household rather than an individual of interest), they can be considered superior to self-report measures because they are not subject to reporting bias.

The aim of the present review was to further consider the likely true effect of tailored nutrition education by assessing the findings of tailored nutrition education intervention trials where objective outcome measures have been employed.

Methods

Randomised controlled trials (RCT) of tailored nutrition education employing objective outcome measures were identified from previous systematic literature reviews(Reference Eyles and Ni Mhurchu2Reference Enwald and Huotari5) and an updated literature search (to December 2010) using a previously published search strategy(Reference Eyles and Ni Mhurchu2). The definition of tailored nutrition education used for this search was that described by Kreuter and colleagues(Reference Kreuter, Strecher and Glassman1). A narrative summary of the methods, findings and quality of trials employing objective outcome measures was undertaken (within the context of previous systematic reviews) to further consider the likely true effect of tailored nutrition education.

Results

Four RCT assessing the effectiveness of tailored nutrition education and employing objective outcome measures of diet were identified: one was undertaken in eight low-cost supermarkets in New Zealand (2010; n 1104)(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6); one was an online intervention trial in Australia (2006; n 497)(Reference Huang, Barzi and Huxley7); and two were undertaken in supermarkets in the USA (1997 and 2001; n 105 and 296, respectively)(Reference Winett, Anderson and Bickley8, Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). The New Zealand and Australian trials used electronic supermarket sales data linked to a food and nutrient database to tailor the nutrition education intervention and assess trial outcomes. The US trials used manually entered supermarket till receipts linked to a food and nutrient database. The study characteristics, findings and quality of these trials are summarised in Table 1, and briefly as follows.

Table 1 Methods and findings of randomised controlled trials of tailored nutrition education that used objective outcome measures

RCT, randomised controlled trial; %TE, percentage of total energy; 0, no difference between groups; +, positive effected of tailored nutrition education; −, negative effect of tailored nutrition education; Y, yes; ITT, intention to treat; NR, not reported; N, no.

Characteristics of included trials

All tailored nutrition education intervention trials employing objective outcome measures compared tailored nutrition education v. no education(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6, Reference Winett, Anderson and Bickley8, Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9), except for the Australian trial(Reference Huang, Barzi and Huxley7) which compared tailored v. generic nutrition education.

The New Zealand trial delivered packages of food-group-‘themed’ information to intervention participants by post(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6); the Australian online trial provided participants with healthier alternatives for selected products containing > 1 % of total energy from saturated fat(Reference Huang, Barzi and Huxley7); and the US trials delivered 5-min education sessions through a kiosk housed in the supermarket. The US interventions also included discount coupons for healthier products(Reference Winett, Anderson and Bickley8, Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). Intensity of the intervention was substantially higher for the US trials compared with the Australian and New Zealand trials (fourteen or fifteen weekly sessions compared with a mean of three online shopping episodes and seven packages of mailed information, respectively).

The intervention period was 6 months for all trials, with the exception of the Australian online shopping trial which lasted for 5 months(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). The New Zealand trial also followed up at 12 months, but the primary outcome (change in purchases of saturated fat) was measured at 6 months(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6). Further, the 2001 US trial also followed up at 15 weeks, but did not specify whether this time point or the 6-month follow-up was the primary measure.

Participants in all four trials were primary household shoppers, predominantly female (range: 86 % for the New Zealand trial(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6) to 96 % for the 2001 US trial(Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9)), and had a mean age of approximately 40 years(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). The New Zealand trial included the greatest proportion of participants from priority population groups (range: 3 %(9) to 32 %(6)).

The most commonly measured outcomes were change in purchases of fruit and vegetables, total fat, and fibre (three trials; Table 1)(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9).

Findings and quality of included trials

The primary outcome of the New Zealand trial was change in purchases of percentage energy from saturated fat at 6 months(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6). The findings of this trial were negative: at 6 months, the difference in saturated fat purchased by tailored nutrition education v. control was −0·09 % of total energy (95 % CI −0·47, 0·30 %; P = 0·66). The authors of this trial also measured a range of other food and nutrient outcomes at 6 and 12 months (Table 1). However, findings for these outcomes were also non-significant. This trial was a high-quality trial conducted and reported according to the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials(Reference Schulz, Altman and Moher10), and loss to follow-up was low (7 %; Table 1). Consequently, it is unlikely that the methods were subject to biases that resulted in the tailored nutrition education intervention appearing unjustly ineffective.

The primary outcome of the Australian online shopping trial was similar to that of the New Zealand trial: change in purchases of percentage energy from saturated fat at 5 months(Reference Huang, Barzi and Huxley7). However, in contrast, the findings were positive: at 5 months, the difference in saturated fat purchased by tailored nutrition education v. generic nutrition education was −0·66 % of total energy (95 % CI 0·48, 0·84 %; P < 0·0 0 1). The findings of this trial are likely to be a true positive effect of tailored nutrition education as this was a high-quality trial and loss to follow-up was low (8 %; Table 1). However, the findings have limited generalisability because relatively few people shop online for groceries, and those who do are generally more highly educated and have higher incomes than in-store shoppers(Reference Kempiak and Fox11, Reference Morganosky and Cude12).

The US trial undertaken by Anderson and colleagues in 2001 measured three outcomes – percentage energy from total fat, fibre (g/4184 kJ (1000 kcal)) and fruit and vegetables (servings/4184 kJ (1000 kcal)) – at 15 weeks and 6 months, but did not specify which of these (if any) was the primary outcome(Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). They found a positive effect of tailoring on total fat and fibre at 15 weeks, but this effect had disappeared at the 6-month follow-up(Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). Reporting of this trial was poor making it difficult to assess study quality (Table 1). However, loss to follow-up was higher than for the New Zealand and Australian trials (13 %).

Finally, the US trial undertaken by Winnett et al.(Reference Winett, Anderson and Bickley8) in 1997 measured eleven outcomes focused on fruit and vegetables and total fat and fibre sourced from various foods (Table 1). The primary outcome was not specified. Of eleven outcomes measured, a positive effect of tailoring was found only for total fat from all foods minus beverages, total fat from dairy products, and fibre from all foods(Reference Winett, Anderson and Bickley8). Similar to the 2001 US study, loss to follow-up was high (28 %) and poor reporting makes it difficult to assess the quality of this trial (Table 1).

Discussion

The present review does not provide support for tailored nutrition education as a stand-alone intervention for improving dietary habits. Four RCT employing objective outcome measures were identified through comprehensive literature searching(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9), three of which reported negative results at the 6-month follow-up(Reference Ni Mhurchu, Blakely and Jiang6, Reference Winett, Anderson and Bickley8, Reference Anderson, Winett and Wojcik9). These findings are in contrast to those of previous systematic literature reviews where included trials have employed predominantly subjective outcome measures(Reference Eyles and Ni Mhurchu2Reference Enwald and Huotari5). The one trial included in the present review that did find a positive effect of tailored nutrition education was undertaken in a sample of online supermarket shoppers and thus has limited generalisability as a strategy for improving dietary habits in large population groups(Reference Huang, Barzi and Huxley7).

It is unlikely that ineffective tailoring frameworks were responsible for the negative findings of the trials included in the present review as the frameworks were similar to those used in positive RCT with subjective outcome measures(Reference Eyles and Ni Mhurchu2). Furthermore, the negative findings are despite the fact that for three of the four included trials feedback was provided at the supermarket where decision making regarding food purchases occurs; two trials included goal setting (a traditionally effective behaviour change construct(Reference Webb and Sheeran13)); and two trials provided participants with discount coupons for healthier foods.

It is notable that the US trial undertaken by Anderson and colleagues in 2001 found a positive effect of tailoring for two of three outcomes measured at 15 weeks (but not at 6 months), and the US trial by Winnett and colleagues (1997) found a positive effect of tailoring for three of eleven outcomes measured (at 6 months). Due to poor reporting of both of these trials it is difficult to assess whether these are true, positive effects of tailoring: both of these trials failed to report which outcome (if any) was the primary and had higher losses to follow-up than the New Zealand and Australian trials (13 % and 28 % compared with 7 % and 8 %, respectively). The positive findings of Winnett et al. in particular could be due to chance effects resulting from the large number of outcomes assessed: including multiple outcomes or endpoints in a study increases the chances of type I error (false positive) because several significant results can be expected to occur by chance alone(Reference Schulz and Grimes14).

The strengths of the present review include the rigorous methodology and use of a comprehensive peer-reviewed search strategy(Reference Eyles and Ni Mhurchu2). However, a limitation is that dietary intervention trials employing objective outcome measures outside of diet were not included (e.g. body weight and cholesterol). Nevertheless, including these trials would have been unlikely to have affected the overall findings: a systematic review published in 2007 by Gorber et al.(Reference Gorber, Tremblay and Moher15) compared self-report with objective measures for assessing height, weight and BMI. Sixty-four trials of adults (> 18 years) were included. Overall, trends were found for under-reporting of weight and BMI, and over-reporting of height. The findings of the review by Gorber et al. thus strengthen the findings of the present review, providing further evidence that self-report measures are inadequate for assessing dietary intervention trials.

The four trials of tailored nutrition education included in the present review do not provide substantive evidence that tailored nutrition education is ineffective across all settings and populations, although they do suggest that tailored nutrition education may not be an effective stand-alone dietary intervention strategy. Regardless, some degree of supportive nutrition education remains warranted, particularly for ethnic minority and vulnerable groups. This is because nutrition education provides the context for dietary interventions, i.e. it tells people why they need to make dietary changes. Nutrition education should be included as a component of broader approaches to improving population diets(Reference Margetts16), such as pricing incentives, social marketing, food labeling, food reformulation and regulation. This conclusion is in line with the findings of a recently published paper regarding the healthfulness of the US food supply: despite decades of dietary education through guidelines, the US food supply has not improved(Reference Krebs-Smith, Reedy and Bosire17). The authors of the US paper conclude that policy makers, the agriculture sector and the food industry should make deliberate efforts to provide a supply of foods consistent with nutrition recommendations. Broader approaches to public health have recently become popular(Reference Reynolds, Klepp and Yaroch18) and are consistent with a balanced public health paradigm where both individual lifestyle behaviours and factors associated with the wider environment are considered responsible for poor nutrition(Reference Margetts16).

Due to the limited quantity and poor quality of two of the four trails included in the present review, further large, high-quality trials of tailored nutrition education employing objective outcome measures are warranted. However, consideration should also be given as to whether other broader, higher-level approaches may be a more appropriate focus for future nutrition research. Regardless of the specific components included, all future nutrition interventions should use objective measures of diet (such as sales data or biomarkers) in combination with more traditional dietary assessment tools. Another important finding of the review is that the quality of two of the four included studies was suboptimal, largely reflecting poor reporting of methods. All researchers of future nutrition trials should thus ensure their reports are consistent with the requirements of the Consolidated Statement on Reporting of Trials(Reference Schulz, Altman and Moher10). In addition to including objective outcome measures, this would ensure that future research can contribute more effectively to the evidence, and produce results that are more useful for nutrition researchers, policy and practice.

Acknowledgements

Sources of funding: H.E. was supported by a Heart Foundation of New Zealand postgraduate scholarship (Grant no. 1285) at the time of writing. C.N.M. holds the Heart Foundation of New Zealand Senior Fellowship (Grant no. 1380). Conflicts of interest: None declared. Author contributions: H.E. was responsible for developing the tailored nutrition education programme for the SHOP (Supermarket Healthy Options Project) trial; completing the literature search, analysis and interpretation; and drafting the manuscript. C.N.M. was responsible for the conception, design, analysis and interpretation of the SHOP trial results; and provided input into the manuscript and interpretation of the literature review findings.

References

1.Kreuter, MW, Strecher, VJ & Glassman, B (1999) One size does not fit all: the case for tailoring print materials. Ann Behav Med 21, 276283.Google Scholar
2.Eyles, H & Ni Mhurchu, C (2009) Does tailoring matter? A systematic review of the long-term effectiveness of tailored nutrition education for adults. Nutr Rev 67, 464480.Google Scholar
3.Kroeze, W, Werkman, A & Brug, J (2006) A systematic review of randomized trials on the effectiveness of computer-tailored education on physical activity and dietary behaviours. Ann Behav Med 31, 205223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4.Noar, SM, Benac, CN & Harris, MS (2007) Does tailoring matter? Meta-analytic review of tailored print health behavior change interventions. Psychol Bull 133, 673693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Enwald, HPK & Huotari, MA (2010) Preventing the obesity epidemic by second generation tailored health communication: an interdisciplinary review. J Med Internet Res 12, e24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Ni Mhurchu, C, Blakely, T, Jiang, Y et al. (2010) Effects of price discounts and tailored nutrition education on supermarket purchases: a randomized, controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 736747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Huang, A, Barzi, F, Huxley, R et al. (2006) The effects on saturated fat purchases of providing internet shoppers with purchase-specific dietary advice: a randomised trial. PLoS Clin Trials 22, e22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8.Winett, RA, Anderson, ES, Bickley, PG et al. (1997) Nutrition for a lifetime system: a multimedia system for altering food supermarket shoppers’ purchases to meet nutritional guidelines. Comput Hum Behav 13, 371392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Anderson, ES, Winett, RA, Wojcik, JR et al. (2001) A computerized social cognitive intervention for nutrition behavior: direct and mediated effects on fat, fiber, fruits, and vegetables, self-efficacy, and outcome expectations among food shoppers. Ann Behav Med 23, 88100.Google Scholar
10.Schulz, KF, Altman, DG & Moher, D (2010) CONSORT 2010 Statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ 320, c332; doi:10.1136/bmj.c332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11.Kempiak, M & Fox, MA (2002). Online grocery shopping: consumer motives, concerns, and business models. http://131.193.153.231/www/issues/issue7_9/kempiak/ (accessed March 2010).Google Scholar
12.Morganosky, M & Cude, B (2000) Consumer response to online grocery shopping. Int J Retail Distribut Manage 28, 1726.Google Scholar
13.Webb, TL & Sheeran, P (2006) Does changing behavioural intentions engender behaviour change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull 131, 249268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14.Schulz, KF & Grimes, DA (2005) Multiplicity in randomised trials I: endpoints and treatments. Lancet 365, 15911595.Google Scholar
15.Gorber, SC, Tremblay, M, Moher, D et al. (2007) A comparison of direct vs. self-report measures for assessing height, weight and body mass index: a systematic review. Obes Rev 8, 307326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.Margetts, BM (2004) Public health nutrition strategies for intervention at the individual level. In Public Health Nutrition, pp. 120132 [MJ Gibney, BM Margetts, JM Kearney et al., editors]. Oxford: Blackwell Science.Google Scholar
17.Krebs-Smith, SM, Reedy, J & Bosire, C (2010) Healthfulness of the US food supply: little improvement despite decades of dietary guidance. Am J Prev Med 38, 472477.Google Scholar
18.Reynolds, KD, Klepp, KI & Yaroch, AL (2004) Public health nutrition strategies for intervention at the ecological level. In Public Health Nutrition, pp. 106119 [ MJ Gibney, BM Margetts, JM Kearney et al., editors]. Oxford: Blackwell Science.Google Scholar
19.Jadad, A (1998) Randomised Controlled Trials. London: BMJ Books.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Methods and findings of randomised controlled trials of tailored nutrition education that used objective outcome measures