Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T04:03:33.163Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multiple cognitive deficits in patients during the mild cognitive impairment stage of Alzheimer's disease: how are cognitive domains other than episodic memory impaired?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 July 2009

Osamu Matsuda*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Educational Psychology, Tokyo Gakugei University, Tokyo, Japan
Masahiko Saito
Affiliation:
Waco Hospital, Saitama, Japan
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Osamu Matsuda, Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Educational Psychology, Tokyo Gakugei University, 4-1-1 Nukuikita Machi, Koganei-shi, Tokyo 184-8501, Japan. Phone: +81.42.329.7373; Fax: +81.42.329.7373. Email: matsuda@u-gakugei.ac.jp.

Abstract

Background: Little is known about how cognitive domains other than episodic memory are affected during the mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage of Alzheimer's disease (AD). We attempted to clarify this issue in this study.

Methods: Fifty-seven Japanese subjects were divided into two groups: one comprising people in the MCI stage of AD (MCI group, n = 28) and the other of normal controls (NC group, n = 29). Cognitive functions were assessed using the Japanese version of the neurobehavioral cognitive status examination (J-COGNISTAT).

Results: The MCI group performed significantly worse than the NC group on subtests that assessed orientation, confrontational naming, constructive ability, episodic memory, and abstract thinking. Three-quarters of the MCI group had deficits in memory and other non-mnemonic domains, particularly constructive ability and abstract thinking. However, within-subject comparisons showed that the MCI group performed significantly worse on the memory subtest compared to any other subtest.

Conclusions: Besides episodic memory, multiple non-mnemonic cognitive domains, such as constructive ability and abstract thinking, are also impaired during the MCI stage of AD; however, these non-mnemonic deficits are smaller than episodic memory impairment.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Arnaiz, E. et al. (2001). Impaired cerebral glucose metabolism and cognitive functioning predict deterioration in mild cognitive impairment. NeuroReport, 12, 851855.Google ScholarPubMed
Bäckman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J. and Small, B. (2004). Multiple cognitive deficits during the transition to Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 195204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, S. B., Zientz, J., Weiner, M., Rosenberg, R., Frawley, W. and Burns, M. H. (2002). Discourse changes in early Alzheimer disease, mild cognitive impairment, and normal aging. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 6, 177186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubois, B. and Albert, M. L. (2004). Amnestic MCI or prodromal Alzheimer's disease? Lancet Neurology, 3, 246248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estévez-González, A. et al. (2004). Semantic knowledge of famous people in mild cognitive impairment and progression to Alzheimer's disease. Dementia Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 17, 188195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. C., Crum, W. R., Scahill, R. I., Stevens, J. M., Janssen, J. J. and Rossor, M. N. (2001). Imaging of onset and progression of Alzheimer's disease with voxel-compression mapping of serial magnetic images. Lancet, 358, 201205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gauthier, S. et al. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet, 367, 12621270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grober, E. et al. (2008). Neuropsychological strategies for detecting early dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 130142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hashimoto, R. et al. (2004). Executive dysfunction can explain word-list learning disability in very mild Alzheimer's disease: the Tajiri project. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 58, 5460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, J. R., Erzinçlioğlu, S. and Patterson, K. (2006). Evolution of cognitive deficits and conversion to dementia in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a very-long-term follow-up study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 380391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, A. S. (1990). Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence (pp. 222231). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Kiernan, R. J., Mueller, J., Langston, J. W. and van Dyke, C. (1987). The neurobehavioral cognitive status examination: a brief but differentiated approach to cognitive assessment. Annals of Internal Medicine, 107, 481485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Killiany, R. J. et al. (2000). Use of structural magnetic resonance imaging to predict who will get Alzheimer's disease. Annals of Neurology, 47, 430439.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kogure, D. et al. (2000). Longitudinal evaluation of early Alzheimer's disease using brain perfusion SPECT. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 41, 11551162.Google ScholarPubMed
Kramer, J. H. et al. (2006). Multiple cognitive deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22, 306311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsuda, O. and Nakatani, M. (2004). Manual for Japanese version of the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination (COGNISTAT). Tokyo: World Planning (in Japanese).Google Scholar
Matsuda, O. and Saito, M. (2005). Functional competency and cognitive ability in mild Alzheimer's disease: relationship between ADL assessed by a relative/carer-rated scale and neuropsychological performance. International Psychogeriatrics, 17, 275288.Google ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G., Drachmann, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D. and Stadian, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’ disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Service Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, J., Kiernan, R. and Langston, J. W. (2007). COGNISTAT manual revised 2007. Fairfax: Northern California Neurobehavioral Group, Inc.Google Scholar
Ozioma, C. et al. (2006). Cognitive correlates of financial abilities in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 54, 17451750.Google Scholar
Petersen, R. C. et al. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Achieves of Neurology, 56, 303308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C. and Morris, J. C. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Archives of Neurology, 62, 11601163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., Kluger, A., Franssen, E., Wegiel, J. and de Leon, M. J. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a historical perspective. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 1831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ribeiro, F., de Mendonca, A. and Guerriro, M. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment: deficits in cognitive domains other than memory. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 284290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taler, V. and Phillips, N. A. (2008). Language performance in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment: a comparative review. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 501556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations (2008). World Population Prospects: The 2006 Revision: Sex and Age Distribution of the World Population. New York: UN Department of Economic and Social Affairs-Population Studies.Google Scholar
van der Flier, W. M. et al. (2002). Magnetization transfer imaging in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Annals of Neurology, 2, 62–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winblad, B. et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment-beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of International Medicine, 256, 240246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamaguchi, H., Sugihara, S., Ogawa, A., Oshima, N. and Ihara, Y. (2001). Alzheimer [beta] amyloid deposition enhanced by ApoE ɛ4 gene precedes neurofibrillary pathology in the frontal association cortex of nondemented senior subjects. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 60, 731739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed