Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T17:53:18.546Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: Implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 November 2005

GERALDINE DAWSON
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability University of Washington
SARA JANE WEBB
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability University of Washington
ELLEN WIJSMAN
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability University of Washington
GERARD SCHELLENBERG
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability Veteran's Affairs Medical Center, Seattle
ANNETTE ESTES
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability University of Washington
JEFFREY MUNSON
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability
SUSAN FAJA
Affiliation:
University of Washington Autism Center and Center on Human Development and Disability University of Washington

Abstract

Neuroimaging and behavioral studies have shown that children and adults with autism have impaired face recognition. Individuals with autism also exhibit atypical event-related brain potentials to faces, characterized by a failure to show a negative component (N170) latency advantage to face compared to nonface stimuli and a bilateral, rather than right lateralized, pattern of N170 distribution. In this report, performance by 143 parents of children with autism on standardized verbal, visual–spatial, and face recognition tasks was examined. It was found that parents of children with autism exhibited a significant decrement in face recognition ability relative to their verbal and visual spatial abilities. Event-related brain potentials to face and nonface stimuli were examined in 21 parents of children with autism and 21 control adults. Parents of children with autism showed an atypical event-related potential response to faces, which mirrored the pattern shown by children and adults with autism. These results raise the possibility that face processing might be a functional trait marker of genetic susceptibility to autism. Discussion focuses on hypotheses regarding the neurodevelopmental and genetic basis of altered face processing in autism. A general model of the normal emergence of social brain circuitry in the first year of life is proposed, followed by a discussion of how the trajectory of normal development of social brain circuitry, including cortical specialization for face processing, is altered in individuals with autism. The hypothesis that genetic-mediated dysfunction of the dopamine reward system, especially its functioning in social contexts, might account for altered face processing in individuals with autism and their relatives is discussed.The writing of this paper and the studies reported herein were funded by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD Grant U19HD34565), which is part of the NICHD Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism, and a center grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH Grant U54MH066399), which is part of the NIH STAART Centers Program.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

August, G. J., Stewart, M. A., & Tsai, L. (1981). The incidence of cognitive disabilities in the siblings of autistic children. British Journal of Psychiatry 138, 416422.Google Scholar
Aylward, E., Bernier, R., Field, K., Grimme, A., & Dawson, G. (2004). Normal activation of fusiform gyrus in adolescents and adults with autism during viewing of familiar faces. Presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Sacramento, CA.
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine 25, 6377.Google Scholar
Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Toward an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines 37, 89126.Google Scholar
Baker, P., Piven, J., Schwartz, S., & Patil, S. (1994). Brief report: Duplication of chromosome 15q11-13 in two individuals with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism Development Disorder 24, 529535.Google Scholar
Baron–Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition 4, 311332.Google Scholar
Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8, 551565.Google Scholar
Bentin, S., & Deouell, L. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cognitive Neuropsychology 17, 3554.Google Scholar
Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2003). The recognition of facial affect in autistic and schizophrenic subjects and their first-degree relatives. Psychological Medicine 33, 907915.Google Scholar
Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., & Rios, P. (1994). A case–control family history study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35, 877900.Google Scholar
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33, 843859.Google Scholar
Boucher, J., Lewis, V., & Collis, G. (1998). Familiar face and voice matching and recognition in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39, 171181.Google Scholar
Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., Metzger, L. M., Shoushtari, C. S., Splinter, R., & Reich, W. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised. Journal of Autism Development and Disorder, 33, 427433.Google Scholar
Constantino, J. N., Hudziak, J. J., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Deficits in reciprocal social behavior in male twins: Evidence for a genetically independent domain of psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry 42, 458467.Google Scholar
Constantino, J. N., Przybeck, T., Friesen, D., & Todd, R. D. (2000). Reciprocal social behavior in children with and without pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 21, 211.Google Scholar
Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2000). Genetic structure of reciprocal social behavior. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 20432045.Google Scholar
Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 524530.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Carver, L. J., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. (2002). Neural correlates of face recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Child Development 73, 700717.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Estes, A., Munson, J., Schellenberg, G., Bernier, R., & Wijsman, E. (2005). Quantitative assessment of autism symptoms in children with autism and their parents: Broader Phenotype Autism Symptom Scale. Unpublished manuscript.
Dawson, G., Hill, D., Galpert, L., Spencer, A., & Watson, L (1990). Affective exchanges between young autistic children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 18, 335345.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 28, 479485.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J., McPartland, J., Toth, K., Carver, L., & Abbott, R. (2002). Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Child Development 73, 345358.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., & Estes, A. (2004). Defining the early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and responses to emotions. Developmental Psychology 40, 271283.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Webb, S., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland, J. (2004). Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions. Developmental Science 7, 340359.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Webb, S., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology 27, 403424. [Special Issue on Autism]Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Webb, S., Schellenberg, G., Aylward, E., Richards, T., Dager, S., & Friedman, S. (2002). Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Genetic, brain, and behavioral perspectives. Development and Psychopathology 14, 581611.Google Scholar
de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1997). Recognition of the mother's face by 6-month old infants: A neurobehavioral study. Child Development 68, 187210.Google Scholar
Delong, G. R., & Dwyer, J. T. (1988). Correlation of family history with specific autistic subgroups: Aspergers and bipolar affective disease. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 18, 593600.Google Scholar
Dorris, L., Espie, C. A. E., Knott, F., & Salt, J. (2004). Mind-reading difficulties in the siblings of people with Asperger's syndrome: Evidence for a genetic influence in the abnormal development of a specific cognitive domain. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 412.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (1998). Does the face specific component reflect the activity of a specialized eye processor? NeuroReport 9, 29452948.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (2000a). Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research 10, 145158.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (2000b). Event related potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition. Clinical Neurophysiology 111, 694705.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (2000c). The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. Cognitive Neuroscience 11, 23192324.Google Scholar
Ferguson, J. N., Young, L. J., Hearn, E., Insel, T. R., & Winslow, J. (2000). Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nature Genetics 25, 284288.Google Scholar
Ferguson, J. N., Young, L. J., & Insel, T. R. (2002). The neuroendocrine basis of social recognition. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 23, 200224.Google Scholar
Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism: A genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 18, 297321.Google Scholar
George, N., Evans, J., Fiori, N., Davidoff, J., & Renault, B. (1996). Brain events related to normal and moderately scrambled faces. Cognitive Brain Research 4, 6576.Google Scholar
Gepner, B., de Gelder, B., & de Schonen, S. (1996). Face processing in autistics: Evidence for a generalized deficit? Child Neuropsychology 2, 123139.Google Scholar
Gepner, B., de Schonen, S., & Buttin, C. (1994). Face processing in young autistic children. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 661.Google Scholar
Giannakopoulos, P., Gold, G., Duc, M., Michel, J. P., Hof, P. R., & Bouras, C. (2003). Impaired processing of famous faces in Alzheimer's disease is related to neurofibrillary tangle densities in the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 11, 336341.Google Scholar
Gingrich, B., Liu, Y., Cascio, C., Wang, Z., & Insel, T. R. (2000). Dopamine D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens are important for social attachment in female prairie voles. Behavioral Neuroscience 114, 173183.Google Scholar
Grelotti, D., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology 40, 213225.Google Scholar
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., Chabris, C. F., Clark, J., Steele, S., McGrath, L., Vangel, M., Aharon, I., Feczko, E., Harris, G. J., & Tager–Flusberg, H. (2004). Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. NeuroImage 22, 11411150.Google Scholar
Happe, F., Briskman, J., & Frith, U. (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak central conference in parents and sibling of children with autism: I. Experimental test. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 299307.Google Scholar
Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Maisog, J. M., Pietrini, P., & Grady, C. L. (1994). The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: A PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations. Journal of Neuroscience 14, 63366353.Google Scholar
Haxby, J., Ungerleider, L., Clark, B., Schouten, J., Hoffman, E., & Martin, A. (1999). The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 22, 189199.Google Scholar
Herrmann, M. J., Ellgring, H., & Hallgatter, A. J. (2004). Early-stage face processing dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 16, 915917.Google Scholar
Hoffman, E., & Haxby, J. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neuroscience 3, 8084.Google Scholar
Holden, C. (2003). Deconstructing schizophrenia. Science 299, 333335.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Plumet, M. H., & Leboyer, J. (1999). Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: Increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40, 705718.Google Scholar
Insel, T. R. (1997). A neurobiological basis of social attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 726735.Google Scholar
Insel, T. R., & Hulihan, T. J. (1995). A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: Oxytocin and partner preference formation in monogamous voles. Behavioral Neuroscience 109, 782789.Google Scholar
Insel, T. R., & Fernald, R. D. (2004). How the brain processes social information: Searching for the social brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27, 697722.Google Scholar
Insel, T. R., O'Brien, D. J., & Leckman, J. F. (1999). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: Is there a connection. Biological Psychiatry 45, 145157.Google Scholar
Jorde, L. B., Hasstedt, S. J., Ritvo, E. R., Mason–Brothers, A., Freeman, B. J., Pingree, C., McMahon, W. M., Peterson, B., Jenson, W. R., & Mo, A. (1991). Complex segregation analysis of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics 49, 932938.Google Scholar
Jorde, L. B., Mason–Brothers, A., Waldmann, R., & Ritvo, E. R. (1990). The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiology survey of autism: Genealogical analysis of familial aggregation. American Journal of Medical Genetics 36, 8588.Google Scholar
Kampe, K., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, U. (2001). Attraction and gaze—The reward value of social stimuli. Nature, 413, 589.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N. (2000). Domain specificity in face perception. Nature Neuroscience 3, 759763.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience 17, 43024311.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., Tong, F., & Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition 68, B1B11.Google Scholar
Karmiloff–Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2, 389399.Google Scholar
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20, 87100.Google Scholar
Kim, S. J., Young, L. J., Gonen, D., Veenstra–VanderWeele, J., Courchesne, R., Courchesne, E., Lord, C., Leventhal, B. L., Cook, E. H., Jr., & Insel, T. R. (2002). Transmission disequilibrium testing of arginine vasopressin receptor 1A (AVPR1A) polymorphisms in autism. Molecular Psychiatry 7, 503507.Google Scholar
Klin, A., Sparrow, S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D., Cohen, D., & Volkmar, F. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29, 499508.Google Scholar
Klinger, L., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in children with autism and Down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology 13, 111124.Google Scholar
Koczat, D. L., Rogers, S. J., Pennington, B. F., & Ross, R. G. (2002). Eye movement abnormality suggestive of a spatial working memory deficit is present in parents of autistic probands. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32, 513518.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277, 684686.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Coffey–Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2004). Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Developmental Science 7, 1930.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100, 90969101.Google Scholar
Kuntsi, L. K., Coleman, J., Campbell, R., & Skuse, D. (2003). Face and emotion recognition deficits in Turner syndrome: A possible role of X-linked genes in amygdala development. Neuropsychology 17, 3949.Google Scholar
Landa, R., Folstein, S., & Isaacs, C. (1991). Spontaneous narrative discourse performance of parents of autistic individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34, 13391345.Google Scholar
Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M. M., Gayle, J. O., Chase, G. A., & Folstein, S. E. (1992). Social language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychological Medicine 22, 245254.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30, 205223.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 659685.Google Scholar
Maurer, D., & Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Development 47, 523527.Google Scholar
McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. (1997). Face specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9, 604609.Google Scholar
McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S., Panagiotides, H., & Carver, L. (2004). Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 12351245.Google Scholar
Modahl, C., Green, L., Fein, D., Morris, M., Waterhouse, L., Feinstein, C., & Levin, H. (1998). Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biological Psychiatry 43, 270277.Google Scholar
Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Gaze following and language development in six-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development 21, 373377.Google Scholar
Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review 2, 164181.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of nonverbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 27, 657669.Google Scholar
Narayan, S., Moyes, B., & Wolff, S. (1990). Family characteristics of autistic children: A further report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20, 523535.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A. (2001). The development and neural bases of face recognition. Infant and Child Development 10, 318.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & de Haan, M. (1996). A neurobiological approach to the recognition of facial expressions in infancy. In J. A. Russell (Ed.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 176204). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nishimori, K., Young, L. J., Guo, Q., Wang, Z., Insel, T. R., & Matzuk, M. M. (1996). Oxytocin is required for nursing but is not essential for parturition or reproductive behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America 93, 1169911704.Google Scholar
Pedersen, C. A., Caldwell, J. O., Walker, C., Ayers, G., & Mason, G. A. (1994). Oxytocin activates the postpartum onset of rat maternal behavior in the ventral tegmental and medial preoptic areas. Behavioral Neuroscience 108, 11631171.Google Scholar
Pickles, A., Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Sim, C.-H, & Rutter, M. (1995). Latent-class analysis of recurrence risks for complex phenotypes with selection and measurement error: A twin and family history study of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics 57, 717726.Google Scholar
Pierce, K., Muller, R., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform “face area” in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain 124, 20592073.Google Scholar
Piven, J., & Palmer, P. (1997). Cognitive deficits in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38, 10111021.Google Scholar
Piven, J., Palmer, P., Jacobi, D., Childress, D., & Arndt, S. (1997). Broader autism phenotype: Evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 185190.Google Scholar
Piven, J., Palmer, P., Landa, R., Santangelo, S., Jacobi, D., & Childress, D. (1997). Personality and language characteristics in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. American Journal of Medical Genetics 74, 398411.Google Scholar
Puce, A., Allison, T., Asgari, M., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: A functional MRI study. Journal of Neuroscience 16, 52055215.Google Scholar
Puce, A., Allison, T., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1995). A functional MRI study of face perception in extrastriate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 74, 11921199.Google Scholar
Puce, A., Smith, A., & Allison, T. (2000). ERPs evoked by viewing facial movements. Cognitive Neuropsychology 17, 221239.Google Scholar
Rebai, M., Poiroux, S., Bernard, C., & Lalonde, R. (2001). Event-related potentials for category-specific information during passive viewing of faces and objects. International Journal of Neuroscience 106, 209226.Google Scholar
Risch, N., Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L., Nouri, N., Hinds, D., Hallmayer, J., Kalaydjieva, L., McCague, P., Dimiceli, S., Pitts, T., Nguyen, L., Yang, J., Harper, C., Thorpe, D., Vermeer, S., Young, H., Hebert, J., Lin, A., Ferguson, J., Chiotti, C., Wieseslater, S., Rogers, T., Salmon, B., Nicholas, P., Petersen, P. B., Pingree, C., McMahon, W., Wong, D. L., Cavallisforza, L. L., Kraemer, H. C., & Myers, R. M. (1999). A genomic screen of autism: Evidence for a multilocus etiology. American Journal of Human Genetics 65, 493507.Google Scholar
Ritvo, E. R., Freeman, B. J., Mason–Brothers, A., Mo, A., & Ritvo, A. (1985). Concordance for the syndrome of autism in 40 pairs of afflicted twins. American Journal of Psychiatry 142, 7477.Google Scholar
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (1999). Emerging self-exploration by 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 206218.Google Scholar
Rossion, B., Gauthier, I., Tarr, M., Despland, P., Bruyer, R., Linotte, S., & Crommelinck, M. (2000). The N170 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: An electrophysiological account of face-specific processes in the human brain. Cognitive Neuroscience 11, 6974.Google Scholar
Rüsseler, J. (2003). Recognition memory for unfamiliar faces does not differ for adult normal and dyslexic readers: An event-related brain potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology 114, 12851291.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Bolton, P., & Le Couteur, A. (1993). Autism: Syndrome definition and possible genetic mechanisms. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture and psychology (pp. 269284). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Samaria, F., & Harter, A. (1994, December). Parameterisation of a stochastic model for human face identification. Presented at the 2nd IEEE Workshop on Applications of Computer Vision, Sarasota, FL.
Schoenbaum, G., Setlow, B., Saddoris, M. P., & Gallagher, M. (2003). Encoding predicted outcome and acquired value in orbitofrontal cortex during cue sampling depends upon input from basolateral amygdala. Neuron 39, 731733.Google Scholar
Schultz, R., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R., Anderson, A., Volkmar, F., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., Cohen, D., & Gore, J. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 331340.Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 80, 127.Google Scholar
Sergent, J., Ohta, S., & Macdonald, B. (1992). Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing: A positron emission tomography study. Brain 115, 1536.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development 63, 796807.Google Scholar
Smalley, S. L., Asarnow, R. F., & Spence, A. (1988). Autism and genetics. Archives of General Psychiatry 45, 953961.Google Scholar
Steffenburg, S., Gillberg, C., Hellgren, L., Andersson, L., Gillberg, I., Jakobsson, G., & Bohman, M. (1989). A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway, and Sweden. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30, 405416.Google Scholar
Strauss, M. (2004, May). Facial expertise: The perception and recognition of faces by infants, preschoolers, and adults with autism. In Current research and hypotheses about the face processing abilities of individuals with autism. Paper presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research. Sacramento, CA.
Sung, Y. J., Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Schellenberg, G. D., & Wijsman, E. (2005). Genetic investigation of quantitative traits related to autism: Use of multivariate polygenic models with ascertainment adjustment American Journal of Human Genetics 76, 6881.Google Scholar
Symons, L., Hains, S., & Muir, S. (1998). Look at me: 5-month-old infant's sensitivity to very small deviations in eye-gaze during social interactions. Infant Behavior and Development 21, 531536.Google Scholar
Tager–Flusberg, H., Plesa–Skwerer, D., Faja, S., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). People with Williams syndrome process faces holistically. Cognition 89, 1124.Google Scholar
Tantam, D., Monagham, L., Nicholson, J., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children's ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30, 623630.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M. (1993). Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: The geodesic sensor net. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 87, 154163.Google Scholar
Waterhouse, L., Fein, D., & Modahl, C. (1996). Neurofunctional mechanisms in autism. Psychological Review 103, 457489.Google Scholar
Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Panagiotides, H. (2005). Recognition memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: Developmental change in ERP responses. Unpublished raw data.
Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Panagiotides, H. (in press). ERP evidence of atypical face processing in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.
Winslow, J. T., Hastings, N., Carter, C. S., Harbaugh, C. R., & Insel, T. R. (1993). A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in monogamous prairie voles. Nature 365, 545548.Google Scholar
Winslow, J. T., & Insel, T. R. (2002). The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides 36, 221229.Google Scholar
Witt, D. M., Winslow, J. T., & Insel, T. R. (1992). Enhanced social interactions in rats following chronic, centrally infused oxytocin. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 43, 855861.Google Scholar
Wojciulik, E., Kanwisher, N., & Driver, J. (1998). Covert visual attention modulates face specific activity in the human fusiform gyrus: fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology 79, 15741578.Google Scholar
Wolff, S., Narayan, S., & Moyes, B. (1988). Personality characteristics of parents of autistic children: A controlled study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 29, 143153.Google Scholar