Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T22:50:32.943Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Referential mapping: A technique for attaching functional significance to the innovative utterances of an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Irene M. Pepperberg*
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
*
Department of Anthropology and the Program on Language and Cognition, Northwestern University, 1810 Hinman Ave., Evanston, IL 60208

Abstract

A set of procedures called referential mapping has been used to assign meaning to spontaneous vocalizations of an African Grey parrot. These spontaneous vocalizations were combinations and phonological variations of specific vocal English labels that the bird had previously acquired in a study on interspecies communication. These recombinations were not necessarily intentionally used to describe or request novel objects or circumstances, even though our earlier data demonstrated that the bird could use vocalizations referentially and intentionally to identify, request, refuse, and categorize various objects. The procedures for dealing with this bird's spontaneous modifications neither attempted to nor needed to evaluate the intentionality of his behavior. Rather, the procedures instructed his trainers to respond to the novel speech acts as though he were intentionally commenting about or requesting objects, actions, or information. The success of such procedures in attaching functional significance to his spontaneous vocalizations and in encouraging subsequent innovation suggests that the technique might be applicable to intervention programs for humans with specific communicative deficits.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baptista, L. F. (1983). Commentary, bird song learning: Theme and variations. In Brush, A. H. & Clark, G. A. Jr, (Eds.), Perspectives in ornithology (pp. 499506). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berko-Gleason, J. (1977). Talking to children: Some notes on feedback. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children (pp. 199205). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Blank, M. (1974). Cognitive function of language in the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 10, 229245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowmaker, J. K. (1986, June). Avian color vision and the environment. Paper presented at International Ornithological Congress (Abst.), Ottawa, Canada.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41, 1104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, I. (1976). Role of referent concreteness in the acquisition of passive sentence comprehension through abstract modeling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22, 185199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1958). Words and things. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Calculator, S. N. (1985). Describing and treating discourse problems in mentally retarded children: The myth of mental retardese. In Ripich, D., Newberry, D., & Spinelli, F. (Eds.), School discourse problems (pp. 125147). San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J., & Miller, G. (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 264293). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carr, E., & Kologinsky, E. (1983). Acquisition of sign language by autistic children. II: Spontaneity and generalization effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 297314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1982). The young word-maker: A case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 390425). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, R. A. (1978). The transition from action to gesture. In Lock, A. (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 231257). London: Academic.Google Scholar
Courtright, J. A., & Courtright, I. C. (1979). Imitative modeling as a language intervention strategy: The effects of two mediating variables. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 389402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1983). Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The “Panglossian paradigm” defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 343390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1978). Language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dollaghan, C. A. (1987). Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 218222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay, W. H., & Schuler, A. L. (1980). Emerging language in autistic children. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Fey, M. (1986). Language intervention in young children. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Fouts, R. S. (1974). Language: Origins, definition, and chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 475482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V., & Rodman, P. (1974). An introduction to language. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Fulwiler, R. L., & Fouts, R. S. (1976). Acquisition of American Sign Language by a noncommunicating autistic child. Journal of Autism and Child Schizophrenia, 6, 4351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furrow, D., & Nelson, K. (1986). A further look at the motherese hypothesis: A reply to Gleitman, Newport, and Gleitman. Journal of Child Language, 13, 163176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, B. T, & Gardner, R. A. (1971). Two-way communication with an infant chimpanzee. In Schrier, A. & Stollnitz, F. (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. 4, pp. 117184). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L. E. (1988). Improving the social-conversational skills of developmentally-delayed children: An intervention study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 156167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11, 4379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, D. R. (1984). Animal thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grosslight, J. H., & Zaynor, W. C. (1967). Verbal behavior in the mynah bird. In Salzinger, K. & Salzinger, S. (Eds.), Research in verbal behavior and some neurophysiological implications (pp. 519). New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harré, R. (1984). Vocabularies and theories. In Harré, R. & Reynolds, V. (Eds.), The meaning of primate signals (pp. 90110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, R. (1984). Must monkeys mean? In Harré, R. & Reynolds, V. (Eds.), The meaning of primate signals (pp. 116137). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Herman, L., Richards, D., & Wolz, J. (1984). Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition, 16, 129219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L. (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium (pp. 331368). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A. G. (1982). Overextensions and underextensions: How different are they? Journal of Child Language, 9, 243247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlan, G. R., Lentz, A., Brenn-White, B., Hodur, P., Egger, D., & Frankoff, D. (1982). Establishing generalized, productive verb-noun phrase usage in a manual language system with moderately handicapped children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroodsma, D. E. (1974). Song learning, dialects, and dispersal in the Bewick's wren. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 35, 352380.Google ScholarPubMed
Kroodsma, D. E. (1988). Song types and their use: Developmental flexibility of the male blue-winged warbler. Ethology, 79, 235247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1983). Crib speech and language play. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Chapman, K., Rowan, L., & Weiss, A. (1983). Three hypotheses concerning young children's imitations of lexical items. Developmental Psychology, 19, 591601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Schwartz, R., Chapman, K., Rowan, L., Prelock, P., Terrell, B., Weiss, A., & Messick, C. (1982). Early lexical acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 554564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lock, A. (1978). Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic.Google Scholar
Lock, A. (1980). The guided reinvention of language. London: Academic.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1970). A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 71, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDade, H., Simpson, M., & Lamb, D. (1982). The use of elicited imitation as a measure of expressive grammar: A question of validity. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 1924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messick, C. K. (1984). Phonetic and contextual aspects of the transition to early words. Doctoral dissertation, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN.Google Scholar
Miller, G. (1967). The psychology of communication. New York: Basic Books.Google ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, O. H. (1950). Learning theory and personality dynamics. New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Nelson, D. A. (1985). The syntactic and semantic organization of pigeon guillemot (Cepphus columba) vocal behavior. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 67, 97130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
Parkel, D. A., & Smith, S. T Jr, (1979). Application of computer-assisted language designs. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Hollis, J. H. (Eds.), Language intervention from ape to child (pp. 441464). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1978, March). Object identification by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Paper presented at Midwest Animal Behavior Society Meeting (Abst.), West Lafayette, IN.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 55, 139160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1983a). Cognition in the African Grey parrot: Preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehension of the class concept. Animal Learning and Behavior, 11, 179185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1983b, June). Interspecies communication: Innovative vocalizations of an African Grey parrot. Paper presented at Animal Behavior Society Meeting (Abst.), Lewisburg, PA.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1985). Social modeling theory: A possible framework for understanding avian vocal learning. Auk, 102, 854864.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987a). Conceptual abilities in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Evidence for comprehension of same/different and categories of color, shape, and matter. Animal Learning and Behavior, 15, 423432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987b). Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African Grey parrot: Labeling of cardinal sets. Ethology, 75, 3761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987c). Interspecies communication: A tool for assessing conceptual abilities in the African Grey parrot. In Greenberg, G. & Tobach, E. (Eds.), Language, cognition and consciousness: Integrative levels (pp. 3156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1988a). The importance of social interaction and observation in the acquisition of communicative competence: Possible parallels between avian and human learning. In Zentall, T. R. & Galef, B. (Eds.), Social learning: A comparative approach (pp. 279299). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1988b). An interactive modeling technique for acquisition of communication skills: Separation of “labeling” and “requesting” in a psittacine subject. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 5976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., & Kozak, F. A. (1986). Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Animal Learning and Behavior, 14, 322330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. (1976). Intelligence in ape and man. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1974). teaching visual language to apes and language deficient persons. In Schiefelbusch, H. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (Eds.), Language perspectives: Acquisition, retardation, and intervention (pp. 347376). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, G. W. (1977). What does it take to bootstrap a language? In MacNamara, J. (Ed.), Language, learning and thought (pp. 3745). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Reichle, J., Rogers, R., & Barrett, C. (1984). Establishing pragmatic discriminations among the communicative functions of requesting, rejecting, and commenting in an adolescent. Journal of the Association of the Severely Handicapped, 9, 3136.Google Scholar
Rice, M. (1980). Cognition to language: Categories, word meanings, and training. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Ristau, C. A., & Robbins, D. (1982). Language in the great apes: A critical review. In Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C., & Busnel, M.-C. (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 141255). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Romski, M. A., Sevcik, R. A., & Joyner, S. (1984). Nonspeech communication systems: Implications for language intervention with mentally retarded children. Topics in Language Disorders, 5, 6681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. M., & Gill, T. V. (1977). Lana's acquisition of language skills. In Rumbaugh, D. M. (Ed.), Language learning by a chimpanzee: The Lana project (pp. 165192). New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salzinger, K. (1973). Animal communication. In Dewsbury, D. A. & Rethlingshafer, D. A. (Eds.), Comparative psychology: A modern survey (pp. 161193). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Salzinger, K. (1980). The concept of behavioral mechanism in language. In Mowrer, O. H. (Ed.), Psychology of language and learning (pp. 213232). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Rubert, E. (1986). Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 211235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Rumbaugh, D., & McDonald, K. (1985). Language learning in two species of apes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 9, 653665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiefelbusch, R. L., & Hollis, J. H. (Eds.). (1979). Language intervention from ape to child. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Schuler, A. L. (1979). Echolalia: Issues and clinical application. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 44, 413434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuler, A. L. (1980). A review of intervention techniques. In Fay, W. H. & Schuler, A. L. (Eds.), Emerging language in autistic children (pp. 137164). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Schusterman, R., & Krieger, K. (1986). Artificial language comprehension and size transposition by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 348355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R., & Leonard, L. (1984). Words, objects, and actions in early lexical acquisition. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 119127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R., & Leonard, L. (1985). Lexical imitation and acquisition in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 141149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, W. J. (1986). Signaling behavior: Contributions of different repertoires. In Schusterman, R. J., Thomas, J. A., & Wood, F. G. (Eds.), Dolphin cognition and behavior: A comparative approach (pp. 315330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1979). The role of social interaction in language acquisition. In Collins, W. A. (Ed.), Children's language and communication (pp. 157182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. (1988). A comparative approach to vocal communication. In Leger, D. W. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Comparative perspectives in modern psychology. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Sundberg, M. L. (1983, May). The beginning of human verbal behavior. Paper presented at the Association of Behavior Analysis Convention, Milwaukee, WI.Google Scholar
Sundberg, M. L. (1985, May). Teaching language to the developmentally disabled. Paper presented at the Association of Behavior Analysis Convention, Columbus, OH.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1943). Man and his works. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1988). Vocal-verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (Eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary (pp. 109147). Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
West, M. J., & King, A. P. (1988). Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbird. Nature, 334, 244246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar