Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-xxrs7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T10:39:12.571Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Long-term neuropsychological outcomes of very low birth weight: Associations with early risks for periventricular brain insults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 November 2004

H. GERRY TAYLOR
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University and Rainbow Babies & Children's Hospital, University Hospitals of Cleveland, Cleveland, Ohio
NORI MINICH
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University and Rainbow Babies & Children's Hospital, University Hospitals of Cleveland, Cleveland, Ohio
BARBARA BANGERT
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology & Neurosurgery, Case Western Reserve University and University Hospitals of Cleveland, Cleveland, Ohio
PAULINE A. FILIPEK
Affiliation:
Departments of Pediatrics & Neurology, University of California Irvine Medical Center, Irvine, California
MAUREEN HACK
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University and Rainbow Babies & Children's Hospital, University Hospitals of Cleveland, Cleveland, Ohio

Abstract

Few follow-up studies of children with very low birth weight (VLBW, <1,500 g) have examined neuropsychological sequelae at later ages or neonatal risks as predictors of these outcomes. The present study assessed cognitive skills at mean age 16 years in 48 participants with <750 g birth weight, 47 with 750–1,499 g birth weight, and 52 term-born controls. Our major objectives were to delineate the long-term cognitive consequences of VLBW, and to determine if risks for periventricular brain insults accounted for variations in outcomes. Analysis revealed poorer outcomes for the <750 g group than for term-born controls on nearly all measures, with specific impairments in visual–motor skills, spatial memory, and executive function. Predictors of outcome for participants with VLBW included lower birth weight, lower weight for gestational age, and a longer period of oxygen requirement for chronic lung disease. The longer-term consequences of VLBW are consistent with expectations based on early brain pathology and suggest limitations to functional plasticity. (JINS, 2004, 10, 987–1004.)

Type
SYMPOSIA ARTICLE
Copyright
© 2004 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, P., Anderson, V., Northam, E., & Taylor, H.G. (2000). Standardization of the Contingency Naming Test (CNT) for school-aged children: A measure of reactive flexibility. Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment, 1, 247273.Google Scholar
Anderson, P., Doyle, L.W., & the Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 32643272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P., Doyle, L.W., & the Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (in press). Executive functioning in school-aged children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weights in the 1990s. Pediatrics.
Aram, D.M. & Eisele, J.A. (1992). Plasticity and recovery of higher cognitive functions following early brain injury. In I. Rapin & S.J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology: Volume 6. Child neuropsychology (pp. 7392). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Bachevalier, J. & Malkova, L. (2000). Neuropsychological indices of early medial temporal lobe dysfunction in primates. In H.S. Levin & J. Grafman (Eds.), Cerebral reorganization of function after brain damage (pp. 2748). New York: Oxford University Press.
Bates, E. & Roe, K. (2001). Language development in children with unilateral brain injury. In C.A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 281308). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Beery, K.E. (1989). Revised administration, scoring, and teaching manual for the Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration. Cleveland, OH: Modern Curriculum Press.
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B.F., Porter, D., Taylor, A.K., & Hagerman, R.J. (2001). Profile of cognitive functioning in women with the fragile X mutation. Neuropsychology, 15, 290299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, J.H. & Waber, D.P. (1996). Developmental scoring system for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Botting, N., Powls, A., Cooke, R.W.I., & Marlow, N. (1998). Cognitive and educational outcome of very low birth weight children in early adolescence. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 652660.Google Scholar
Botting, N., Powls, A., Cooke, R.W.I., & Marlow, N. (1997). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and other psychiatric outcomes in very low birthweight children at 12 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 931941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brazy, J.E., Goldstein, R.F., Oehler, J.M., Gustafson, K.E., & Thompson, R.J., Jr. (1993). Nursery Neurobiologic Risk Score: Levels of risk and relationships with nonmedical factors. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 14, 375380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslau, N., Chilcoat, H., Del Dotto, J.E., Andreski, P., & Brown, G.G. (1996). Low birth weight and neurocognitive status at six years of age. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 389387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, N.J., Clark, R.E., Zola, S., & Squire, L.R. (2002). The medial temporal lobe and memory. In L.R. Squire & D.L Schacter (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (3rd ed., pp. 323). New York: Guilford.
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language, manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
CeNeS Cognition. (1996). Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB). CeNeS Limited: Cambridge, UK.
Cole, C., Hagadorn, J., & Kim, C. (2002). Criteria for determining disability in infants and children: Low birth weight. Evidence report/Technology Assessment No. 70, Publication No. 03-E010. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Counsell, S.J., Allsop, J.M., Harrison, M.C., Larkman, D.J., Kennea, N.L., Kapellou, O., Cowan, F.M., Hajnal, J.V., Edwards, A.D., & Rutherford, M.A. (2003). Diffusion-weighted imaging of the brain in preterm infants with focal and diffuse white matter abnormality. Pediatrics, 112, 17.Google Scholar
Dammann, O. & Leviton, A. (1999). Brain damage in preterm newborns: Might enhancement of developmentally regulated endogenous protection open a door for prevention? Pediatrics, 104, 541550.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). CVLT–II: California Verbal Learning Test, Adult Version, manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Denckla, M.B. & Reiss, A.L. (1997). Prefrontal–subcortical circuits in developmental disorders. In N.A. Krasnegor, G.R. Lyon, & P.S. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.), Development of the prefrontal cortex: Evolution, neurobiology, and behavior (pp. 283293). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Dennis, M., Hetherington, C.R., Spiegler, B.J., & Barnes, M.A. (1999). Functional consequences of congenital cerebellar dysmorphologies and acquired cerebellar lesions of childhood. In S.H. Broman & J.M. Fletcher (Eds.), The changing nervous system: Neurobehavioral consequences of early brain disorders (pp. 172198). New York: Oxford University Press.
D'Esposito, M. & Postle, B.R. (2002). The neural basis of working memory storage, rehearsal, and control processes: Evidence from patient and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. In L.R. Squire & D.L Schacter (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (3rd ed., pp. 215224). New York: Guilford.
Durand, M., McEvoy, C., & MacDonald, K. (1992). Spontaneous desaturations in intubated very low birth weight infants with acute and chronic lung disease. Pediatric Pulmonology, 13, 136142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eichenwald, E.C., Aina, A., & Stark, A.R. (1997). Apnea frequently persists beyond term gestation in infants delivered at 24 to 28 weeks. Pediatrics, 100, 354359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espy, K.A., Stalets, M.M., McDiarmid, M.M., Senn, T.E., Cwik, M.F., & Hamby, A. (2002). Executive function in preschool children born preterm: Application of cognitive neuroscience paradigms. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 8392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanaroff, A.A., Hack, M., & Walsh, M.C. (2003). The NICHD Neonatal Research Network: Changes in practice and outcomes during the first 15 years. Seminars in Perinatology, 27, 281287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farel, A.M., Hooper, S.R., Teplin, S.W., Henry, M.M., & Kraybill, E.N. (1998). Very-low-birthweight infants at seven years: An assessment of the health and neurodevelopmental risk conveyed by chronic lung disease. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 118126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, J.M., Landry, S.H., Bohan, T.P., Davidson, K.C., Brookshire, B.L., Lachar, D., Kramer, L.A., & Francis, D.J. (1997). Effects of intraventricular hemorrhage and hydrocephalus on the long-term neurobehavioral development of preterm very-low-birthweight infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 39, 596606.Google Scholar
Foreman, N. Fielder, A., Minshell, C., Hurrion, E., & Sergienko, E. (1997). Visual search, perception, and visual-motor skill in “healthy” children born at 27–32 weeks' gestation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 2741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisk, V. & Whyte, H. (1994). The long-term consequences of periventricular brain damage on language and verbal memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 313333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabrielson, J., Hard, A.-L., Ek, U., Svensson, E., Carlsson, G., & Hellstrom, A. (2002). Large variability in Performance IQ associated with postnatal morbidity, and reduced Verbal IQ among school-aged children born preterm. Acta Paediatrica, 91, 13711378.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. (1979). The objective diagnosis of minimal brain dysfunction. Cresshill, NJ: Creative Therapies.
Garg, M., Kurzner, S.I., Bautista, D.B., & Keens, T.G. (1988). Clinically unsuspected hypoxia during sleep and feeding in infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Pediatrics, 81, 635642.Google Scholar
Golden, C.J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test: Manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago: Stoelting.
Goto, M., Ota, R., Sugita, I.M., & Tanabe, Y. (1994). MRI changes and deficits of higher brain functions in preterm diplegia. Acta Paediatrica, 83, 506511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goyen, T., Lui, K., & Woods, R. (1998). Visual-motor, visual-perceptual, and fine motor outcomes in very-low-birthweight children at 5 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 7681.Google Scholar
Grunau, R.E., Whitfield, M.F., & Davis, C. (2002). Pattern of learning disabilities in children with extremely low birth weight and broadly average intelligence. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 156, 615620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hack, M. (1997). Effects of intrauterine growth retardation on mental performance and behavior: Outcomes during adolescence and adulthood. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 52 (suppl. 1), 6571.Google Scholar
Hack, M. (1999). Consideration of the use of health status, functional outcome and quality-of-life to monitor neonatal intensive care practice. Pediatrics, 103, 319328.Google Scholar
Hack, M., Breslau, N., Aram, D., Weissman, B., Klein, N., & Borawski-Clark, E. (1992). The effect of very low birthweight and social risk on neurocognitive abilities at school age. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 13, 412420.Google Scholar
Hack, M., Flannery, D.J., Schluchter, M., Cartar, L., Borawski, E., & Klein, N. (2002). Outcomes in young adulthood for very-low-birth-weight infants. New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 149157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hack, M., Friedman, H., & Fanaroff, A.A. (1996a). Outcomes of extremely low birthweight infants. Pediatrics, 98, 931937.Google Scholar
Hack, M., Klein, N., & Taylor, H.G. (1996b). School-age outcomes of children of extremely low birth weight and gestational age. Seminars in Neonatology, 1, 227288.Google Scholar
Hack, M., Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Eiben, R., Schatschneider, C., & Mercuri-Minich, N. (1994). School-age outcomes in children with birth weights under 750 g. New England Journal of Medicine, 331, 753759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, J.M., O'Callaghan, M.J., & Mohay, H. (1999). Executive function of children with extremely low birthweight: A case study. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 41, 292297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holling, E.E. (1999). Characteristics of cranial ultrasound white-matter echolucencies that predict disability: A review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 136139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Department of Sociology.
Holsti, L., Grunau, R.V.E., & Whitfield, M.F. (2002). Developmental coordination disorder in extremely low birth weight children at nine years. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23, 915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins-Golightly, T., Raz, S., & Sander, C.J. (2003). Influence of slight to moderate risk for birth hypoxia on acquisition of cognitive and language function in the preterm infant: A cross-sectional comparison with preterm-birth controls. Neuropsychology, 17, 313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horwood, L.J., Mogridge, N., & Darlow, B.A. (1998). Cognitive, educational, and behavioural outcomes at 7 and 8 years in a national very low birthweight cohort. Archives of Diseases of Children, Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 79, F12F20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C.A., O'Gorman, L.A., Shyr, Y., Schork, M.A., Bozynski, M.E.A., & McCormick, M.C. (1999). Cognitive performance at school age of very low birth weight infants with bronchopulmonary dysplagia. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 20, 18.Google Scholar
Hutton, J.L., Pharoah, P.O.D., Cooke, R.W.I., & Stevenson, R.C. (1997). Differential effects of preterm birth and small gestational age on cognitive and motor development. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 76, F75F81.Google Scholar
Inder, T.E., Huppi, P.S., Sarfield, S., Kikinis, R., Zientara, G.P., Barnes, P.D., Jolesz, F., & Volpe, J.J. (1999). Periventricular white matter injury in the premature infant is followed by reduced cerebral cortical gray matter volume at term. Annals of Neurology, 46, 755760.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inder, T.E., Anderson, N.J., Spencer, C., Wells, S.J., & Volpe, J.J. (2003). Periventricular white matter injury in the premature infant: A comparison between serial ultrasound and MRI at term. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 24, 805809.Google Scholar
Inder, T.E. & Volpe, J.J. (2000). Mechanisms of perinatal brain injury. Seminars in Neonatology, 5, 316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, L.S., Frisk, V.A., Knight, R.M., Downie, A.L.S., & Whyte, H. (2001). The relationship between periventricular braininjury and deficits in visual processing among extremely-low-birth-weight (<1000 g) children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26, 503512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. & Applegate, B. (1988). Short forms of K-ABC Mental Processing and Achievement Scales at age 4 to 12-1/2 years for clinical and screening purposes. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 17, 359369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knowlton, B. (2002). The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory. In L.R. Squire & D.L Schacter (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (3rd ed., pp. 143153). New York: Guilford.
Klebanov, P.K., Brooks-Gunn, J., & McCormick, M.C. (1994). School achievement and failure in very low birthweight children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 15, 248256.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Liikanen, A., & Fellman, V. (1996). Neuropsychological consequences of very low birth weight and asphyxia at term: Follow-up until school-age. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 220233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S.H., Fletcher, J.M., Denson, S.E., & Chapieski, M.L. (1993). Longitudinal outcome for low birth weight infants: Effects of intraventricular hemorrhage and bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 205218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, C.H., Clyman, R.I., Piecuch, R.E., Juster, R.P., Ballard, R.A., & Behle, M.B. (1990). Effect of medical and social risk factors on outcome of prematurity and very low birth weight. Journal of Pediatrics, 116, 620626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lou, H.C. (1996). Etiology and pathogenesis of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Significance of prematurity and perinatal hypoxic-haemodynamic encephalopathology. Acta Paediatrica, 85, 12661271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M. (2003). Practitioner review: Computerized assessment of neuropsychological function in children: Clinical and research applications of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 649663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M., Lindeke, L., Georgieff, M., Mills, M., & Nelson, C.A. (1999). Neurobehavioral evidence for working-memory deficits in school-aged children with histories of prematurity. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 521533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M. & Nelson, C.A. (1998). The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children. Neuropsychologia, 36, 273293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M., Sullivan, J., & Nelson, C.A. (2001). Associations between phenylalanine-to-tyrosine ratios and performance on tests of neuropsychological function in adolescents treated early and continuously for phenylketonuria. Child Development, 72, 16371652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luoma, L., Herrgard, E., & Martikainen, A. (1998). Neuropsychological analysis of the visuomotor problems in children born preterm at ≤32 weeks gestation: A 5-year prospective follow-up. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 2130.Google Scholar
Luoma, L., Herrgard, E., Martikainen, A., & Ahonen, T. (1998). Speech and language development of children born at ≤32 weeks gestation: A 5-year prospective follow-up study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 380387.Google Scholar
Maalouf, E.F., Duggan, P.J., Rutherford, M.A., Counsell, S.J., Fletcher, A.M., Battin, M., Cowan, F., & Edwards, A.D. (1999). Magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in a cohort of extremely preterm infants. Journal of Pediatrics, 135, 351357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlow, N., Roberts, B.L., & Cooke, R.W.I. (1989). Motor skills in extremely low birthweight children at the age of 6 years. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 64, 839847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, J.A., Hamilton, B.E., Ventura, S., & Menacker, F. (2002). Births: Final data for 2000. National Vital Statistics Reports, 50, 1104.Google Scholar
McGrath, M. & Sullivan, M. (2002). Birth weight, neonatal morbidities, and school age outcomes in full-term and preterm infants. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 25, 231254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesulam, M. (1985). Principles of behavioral neurology. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis.
Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 313334), New York: McGraw-Hill.
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences and psychological processes. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neville, H.J. & Bavelier, D. (2001). Variability of developmental plasticity. In J.L. McClelland & R.S. Siegler (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives (pp. 271287). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Pennington, B.F. (1994). The working memory function of the prefrontal cortices: Implications for developmental and individual differences in cognition. In M.M. Haith, J. Benson, R. Roberts, & B.F. Pennington (Eds.), The development of future oriented processes (pp. 243289). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Perlman, J.M. (1998). White matter injury in the preterm infant: An important determination of abnormal neurodevelopment outcome. Early Human Development, 53, 99120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, J.M. (2001). Neurobehavioral deficits in premature graduates of intensive care—potential medical and neonatal environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 108, 13391348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, B.S., Vohr, B., Staib., L.H., Cannistraci, C.J., Dolberg, A., Schneider, K.C., Katz, K.H., Westerveld, M., Sparrow, S., Anderson, A.W., Duncan, C.C., Makuch, R.W., Gore, J.C., & Ment, L.R. (2000). Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 19391947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrides, M. & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal and temporal lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pharoah, P.O.D., Stevenson, C.J., & West, C.R. (2003). General certificate of secondary education performance in very low birthweight infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 88, 295298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinto-Martin, J.A., Whitaker, A.H., Feldman, J.F., Van Rossem, R., & Paneth, N. (1999). Relation of cranial ultrasound abnormalities in low-birthweight infants to motor or cognitive performance at ages 2, 6, and 9 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 826833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rickards, A.L., Kelly, E.A., Doyle, L.W., & Callanan, C. (2001). Cognition, academic progress, behavior and self-concept at 14 years of very low birth weight children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, S.A. (1983). Differential rates of visual information processing in fullterm and preterm infants. Child Development, 54, 11891198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, S.A. & Feldman, J.F. (1996). Memory and processing speed in preterm children at eleven years: A comparison with full-terms. Child Development, 67, 20052021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, G., Boatright, S., Auld, P.A.M., & Nass, R. (1996). Specific cognitive abilities in 2-year-old children with subependymal and mild intraventricular hemorrhage. Brain and Cognition, 32, 113.Google Scholar
Ross, G., Tesman, J., Auld, P.A.M., & Nass, R. (1992). Effects of subependymal and mild intraventricular lesions on visual attention and memory in premature infants. Developmental Psychology, 28, 10671074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, S.C., Baudin, J., McCormick, D.C., Edwards, A.D., Townsend, J., Stewart, A.L., & Reynolds, E.O.R. (1993). Relation between ultrasound appearance of the brain of very preterm infants and neurodevelopmental impairment at eight years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 755768.Google Scholar
Rourke, B.P. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities: The syndrome and the model. New York: Guilford.
Saigal, S., Hoult, L.A., Streiner, D.L., Stoskopf, B.L., & Rosenbaum, P.L. (2000). School difficulties at adolescence in a regional cohort of children who were extremely low birthweight. Pediatrics, 105, 325331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, D.P., Heindel, W.C., & Hamilton, J.M. (2001). Cognitive abilities mediated by frontal-subcortical circuits. In D.G. Lichter & J.L. Cummings (Eds.), Frontal–subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 114150). Guilford.
Sattler, J.M. (1992). Assessment of children (revised and updated third edition). San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler.
Schatz, J., Kramer, J.H., Ablin, A., & Matthay, K.K. (2000). Processing speed, working memory, and IQ: A developmental model of cognitive deficits following cranial radiation therapy. Neuropsychology, 14, 189200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selzer, S.C., Lindgren, S.D., & Blackman, J.A. (1992). Long-term neuropsychological outcome of high risk infants with intracranial hemorrhage. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 17, 407422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments in planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B. Biological Sciences, 298, 199209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (2nd Ed.). New York: Oxford.
Stiles, J. (2000). Spatial cognitive development following prenatal or perinatal focal brain injury. In H.S. Levin & J. Grafman (Eds.), Cerebral reorganization of function after brain damage (pp. 201217). New York: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, H.G. & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences in outcomes following childhood brain insults: An introduction and overview. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 555567.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Burant, C.J., Holding, P.A., Klein, N., & Hack, M. (2002). Sources of variability in sequelae of very low birth weight. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 164178.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., & Hack, M. (2000a). School-age consequences of <750 g birth weight: A review and update. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 289321.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Minich, N., & Hack, M. (2000b). Middle school-age outcomes in children with very low birthweight. Child Development, 71, 14951511.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Minich, N., & Hack, M. (2000c). Verbal memory deficits in children with less than 750 g birth weight. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 4963.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Klein, N., Schatschneider, C., & Hack, M. (1998). Predictors of early school age outcomes in very low birthweight children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 19, 235243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Minich, N.M., Klein, N., & Hack, M. (2004). Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: Neuropsychological findings. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 149163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Schatschneider, C., & Minich, N.M. (2000d). Longitudinal outcomes of Haemphilus influenzae meningitis in school-age children. Neuropsychology, 14, 509518.Google Scholar
Thompson, S. (1999). The source for nonverbal learning disorders. East Moline, IL: LinguiSystems.
Tideman, E. (2000). Longitudinal follow-up of children born preterm: Cognitive development at age 19. Early Human Development, 58, 8190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Usher, R. & McLean, F. (1969). Intrauterine growth of live-born Causasian infants at sea level: Standards obtained from measurements of 7 dimensions of infants born between 25 and 44 weeks gestation. Pediatrics, 74, 901910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volpe, J.J. (1998). Brain injury in the premature infant: Overview of clinical aspects, neuropathology, and pathogenesis. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 5, 135151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volpe, J.J. (2001). Neurology of the newborn (4th ed.). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.
Volpe, J.J. (2003). Cerebral white matter injury of the premature infant—More common than you think. Pediatrics, 112, 176179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waber, D.P. & McCormick, M.C. (1995). Late neuropsychological outcomes in preterm infants of normal IQ: Selective vulnerability of the visual system. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 20, 721735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC–III: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition: Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS–III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Whitfield, M.F., Grunau, R.V.E., & Holsti, L. (1997). Extremely premature (≤800 g) schoolchildren: Multiple areas of hidden disability. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 77, F85F90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, S.P., Weinborn, M., Ball, J.D., Tiller-Nevin, S., & Pickett, T.C. (2000). Periventricular leukomalacia (PVL): An identical twin case study illustration of white matter dysfunction and nonverbal learning disability (NLD). Child Neuropsychology, 6, 274285.Google Scholar