Hostname: page-component-6b989bf9dc-jks4b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-14T19:35:42.186Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ventral/dorsal, predicate/argument: The transformation from perception to meaning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2004

James R. Hurford*
Affiliation:
Language Evolution and Computation Research Unit, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, EdinburghEH8 9LL, United Kingdom

Abstract:

It is necessary to distinguish among representations caused directly by perception, representations of past perceptions in long-term memory, the representations underlying linguistic utterances, and the surface phonological and grammatical structures of sentences. The target article dealt essentially with predicate-argument structure at the first of these levels of representation. Discussion of the commentaries mainly involves distinguishing among various applications of the term “predicate”; clarifying the assumed relationship between classical FOPL and language; clarifying the status of unique individuals as conceived by humans; and addressing the issues of motion-perception, binding between object-percepts and predicate-percepts, and target-driven versus stimulus-driven attention.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aglioti, S., Goodale, M. A. & DeSouza, J. F. X. (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology 5:679–85. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. & Chapman, D. (1987) Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity. Proceedings of the AAAI 87:268–72. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Aguirre, G. K. & D’Esposito, M. (1997) Environmental knowledge is subserved by separable dorsal /ventral neural areas. The Journal of Neuroscience 17(7):2512–18. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Aitchison, J. (1996) The seeds of speech. Cambridge University Press. [BI]Google Scholar
Anastasio, T. J., Patton, P. E. & Belkacem-Boussaid, K. (2000) Using Bayes’ rule to model multisensory enhancement in the superior colliculus. Neural Computation 12:9971019. [TJA]Google Scholar
Andersen, R. A., Bracewell, R. M., Barash, S., Gnadt, J. W. & Fogassi, L. (1990) Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7A of macaque. Journal of Neuroscience 10:1176–96. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Andersen, R. A., Essick, G. K. & Siegel, R. M. (1985) Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 230:456–58. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Anderson, S. R. & Keenan, E. L. (1985) Deixis. In: Language typology and syntactic description, vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, ed. Shopen, T. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Antell, S. E. & Keating, J. P. (1983) Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. Child Development 54:695701. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (1972) The metaphorical brain: An introduction to cybernetics as artificial intelligence and brain theory. Wiley-Interscience. [MAA]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (1981) Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In: Handbook of physiology, section 2: The nervous system, vol. 2, motor control, part 1, ed. Brooks, V. B. American Physiological Society. [MAA]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (1987a) Levels of modeling of visually guided behavior (with peer commentary and author's response). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10:407–65. [MAA]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (1987b) Modularity and interaction of brain regions underlying visuomotor coordination. In: Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding, ed. Garfield, J. L. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Érdi, P., & Szentágothai, J. (1998) Neural organization: Structure, function, and dynamics. MIT Press. [MAA]Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. M. (1978) Nominalism and realism: Universals and scientific realism, vol. I. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M. & Pelz, J. B. (1995) Memory representations in natural tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7(1):6680. [aJRH, AK]Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., Pook, P. K. & Rao, R. P. N. (1997) Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20(4):723–67. [IB, aJRH, AK]Google Scholar
Barlow, H. B. (1969) Pattern recognition and the responses of sensory neurons. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 156:872–81. [TJA]Google Scholar
Barlow, H. B. (1972) Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? Perception 1:371–94. [TJA]Google Scholar
Barrett, M. (1995) Early lexical development. In: The handbook of child language, ed. Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. Blackwell. [BI]Google Scholar
Batali, J. (2002) The negotiation and acquisition of recursive grammars as a result of competition among exemplars. In: Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models, ed. Briscoe, T. Cambridge University Press. [MLA, J-LD, aJRH]Google Scholar
Baylis, G. C. (1994) Visual attention and objects: Two-object costs with equal convexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20(1):208–12. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Baylis, G. C. & Driver, J. (1993) Visual attention and objects: Evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 19(3):451–70. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bear, M. F., Connors, B. W. & Paradiso, M. A. (2001) Neuroscience: Exploring the brain. Lippincott. [SL]Google Scholar
Beintema, J. A. & Lappe, M. (2002) Perception of biological motion without local image motion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99(8):5661–63. [rJRH]Google Scholar
Belin, P. & Zatorre, R. J. (2000) ‘What’, ‘where’ and ‘how’ in auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience 3(10):965–66. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bellugi, U, Sabo, H. & Vaid, J. (1988) Spatial deficits in children with Williams syndrome. In: Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development, ed. Stiles-Davis, J., Kritchevsky, M. & Bellugi, U. Erlbaum. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bennett, J. (1976) Linguistic behavior. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Berkeley, G. (1710/1962) The principles of human knowledge. Open Court. [FC]Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1998) Catastrophic evolution: The case for a single step from protolanguage to full human language. In: Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Knight, C. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1990) Language and species. University of Chicago Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bishop, C. M. (1995) Neural networks for pattern recognition. Clarendon Press. [TJA]Google Scholar
Blaser, E., Pylyshyn, Z. W. & Holcombe, A. O. (2000) Tracking an object through feature space. Nature 408:196–99. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973) One word at a time. Morton. [BI]Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000) How children learn the meanings of words: Learning, development and conceptual change. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Borer, H. (in press) Structuring sense. University of Southern California. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (1992) On the origin of consciousness and language. Psycoloquy (refereed electronic journal) March 17, 1992. http://psycprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000239/ [BB]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (1993) Spatial and cognitive vision differentiate at low levels, but not in language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(2):240. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (1999) Vertical modularity in the visual system. In: Stratification in cognition and consciousness, ed. Velichkovsky, B. M. & Challis, B. John Benjamins. [BB]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Gemmer, A., Forsman, T. & Huemer, V. (2000) Properties of the sensorimotor branch of the visual system, Vision Research 40:3539–52. [BB]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G. & Nagle, M. (1979) Relationship between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5:692700. [aJRH, PFM]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., van der Heijden, A. H. C. & Velichkovsky, B. M. (1994) A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 7(2):247–92. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Brinck, I. (2001) Attention and the evolution of intentional communication. Pragmatics and Cognition 9(2):255–72. [IB]Google Scholar
Brooks, R. (1991) Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence 47:139–59. [IB]Google Scholar
Brotchie, P. R., Anderson, R. A., Snyder, L. H. & Goodman, S. J. (1995) Head position signals used by parietal neurons to encode locations of visual stimuli. Nature 375:232–35. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Bryant, D. J. (1993) Frames of reference in the spatial representation system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(2):241–42. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Calvin, W. H. & Bickerton, D. (2000) Lingua ex machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the human brain. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (1994) Past, space, and self. MIT Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Campbell, R. & Woll, B. (2003) Space is special in sign. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:1:57. [BW]Google Scholar
Carnap., R. (1929/1967) The logical structure of the world and pseudoproblems in philosophy. University of California Press. [FC]Google Scholar
Carnap., R. (1939/1949) The logical syntax of language. Routledge. [FC]Google Scholar
Carnap., R. (1942) Introduction to semantics. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F. & Thulborn, K. R. (1999) Timecourse of fMRI activation in language and spatial networks during sentence comprehension. Neuroimage 10:216–24. [BW]Google Scholar
Carrozzo, M., McIntyre, J., Zago, M. & Lacquaniti, F. (1999) Viewer-centered and body-centered frames of reference in direct visuomotor transformations. Experimental Brain Research 129(2):201–10. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1999) The origins of complex language: An inquiry into the evolutionary beginnings of sentences, syllables and truth. Oxford University Press. [AC-M]Google Scholar
Cassirer, E. (1955) The philosophy of symbolic forms, vol. 1: Language, trans. Manheim, R. Yale University Press. [BI]Google Scholar
Chen, J., Myerson, J., Hala, S. & Simon, A. (2000) Behavioral evidence for brainbased ability factors in visuospatial information processing. Neuropsychologia 38:380–87. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990) How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. University of Chicago Press. [AC-M]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1988) Language and interpretation: Philosophical reflections and empirical inquiry. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Clarke, S., Bellmann, A., Meuli, R. A., Assal, G. & Steck, A. J. (2000) Auditory agnosia and auditory spatial deficits following left hemispheric lesions: Evidence for distinct processing pathways. Neuropsychologia 38:797807. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Copi, I. (1958) Artificial languages. In: Language, thought and culture, ed. Henle, P. University of Michigan Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2001) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(1):87114. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Croft, W. (2000) Parts of speech as language universals and as language-particular categories. In: Approaches to the typology of word classes, ed. Vogel, P. M. & Comrie, B. Mouton de Gruyter. [MW]Google Scholar
Cruse, A. (1973) Some thoughts on agentivity. Journal of Linguistics 9:1123. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Culham, J. C. & Kanwisher, N. G. (2001) Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Current Opinion Neurobiology 11:157–63. [BW]Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R. D. & Damasio, A. (1996) A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature 380:499505. [PFM]Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Ponto, L. L. B., Hichwa, R. D. & Damasio, A. R. (2001) Neural correlates of naming actions and naming spatial relations. NeuroImage 13:1053–64. [BW]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (l871) The descent of man. Great Books, Encyclopedia Brittanica. [PFM]Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1980) Essays on actions and events. Clarendon Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
DeGelder, B., Vroomen, J., Pourtois, G. & Weiskrantz, L. (1999) Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. NeuroReport 10(18)3759–63. [aJRH]Google Scholar
DeGelder, B., Vroomen, J., Pourtois, G. & Weiskrantz, L. (2000) Affective blindsight: Are we blindly led by emotions? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(4):126–27. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997) The number sense. Oxford University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
De Mornay Davies, P. & Funnell, E. (2000) Semantic representation and ease of predication. Brain and Language 73:92119. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Dessalles, J.-L (1996) L’ordinateur genetique. Hermes. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Dessalles, J.-L (2000) Aux origines du langage: Une histoire naturelle de la parole. Hermes. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Didday, R. L. & Arbib, M. A. (1971) The organization of action-oriented memory for a perceiving system I: The basic model. Journal of Cybernetics 1:318. [MAA]Google Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. (1980) The languages of Australia. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. (1982) Where have all the adjectives gone?: And other essays in semantics and syntax. Mouton. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Dominey, P. F. (2000) Conceptual grounding in simulation studies of language acquisition. Evolution of Communication 4(1):5785. [PFD]Google Scholar
Dominey, P. F. (2002) Language learning for human-machine interaction via mapping of grammatical structure to visual scene structure. Institut des Sciences Cognitives (ISC) Working Paper 2002–2, http://www.isc.cnrs.fr/wp/DomineyISC.PDF. [PFD]Google Scholar
Dominey, P. F. (2003) Learning grammatical constructions in a miniature language from narrated video events. In: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. (in press). [PFD]Google Scholar
Dominey, P. F., Hoen, M., Blanc, J. M. & Lelekov-Boissard, T. (2003) Neurological basis of language and sequential cognition: Evidence from simulation, aphasia and ERP studies. Brain and Language [PFD]Google Scholar
Douglas, K. (2001) Playing fair. New Scientist 2281:3842. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Dowty, D. (1991) Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 67(3):547619. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Driver, J. & Baylis, G. C. (1998) Attention and visual object segmentation. In: The attentive brain, ed. Parasuraman, R. MIT Press. [IB]Google Scholar
Duda, R. O., Hart, P. E. & Stork, D. G. (2001) Pattern classification. Wiley. [TJA]Google Scholar
Duhamel, J.-R., Colby, C., & Goldberg, M. (1992) The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science 255:9092. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993) Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:681735. [DB]Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (1984) Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113(4):501–17. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Egly, R., Driver, J. & Rafal, R. D. (1994) Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 123(2):161–77. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Eilan, N. (1998) Perceptual intentionality, attention and consciousness. In: Contemporary issues in the philosophy of mind, ed. O’Hear, A. Cambridge University Press. [IB]Google Scholar
Eimer, M., Nattkemper, D., Schröger, E. & Prinz, W. (1996) Involuntary attention. In: Handbook of perception and action, vol. 3: Attention, ed. Sanders, A. & Neumann, O. Academic Press. [IB]Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Corina, D. & Bellugi, U. (1995) Differential processing of topographic and referential functions of space. In: Language, gesture and space, ed. Emmorey, K. & Reilly, J. Erlbaum. [BW]Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Damasio, H., McCullough, S., Grabowski, T., Ponto, L. L. B., Hichwa, R. D. & Bellugi, U. (2002) Neural systems underlying spatial language in American Sign Language. NeuroImage 17:812–24. [BW]Google Scholar
Evans, G. (1982) Varieties of reference. Clarendon. [GG]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975) The language of thought. Crowell. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1987) Psychosemantics. Bradford Books/MIT Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. & Lepore, E. (2002) The compositionality papers. Oxford University Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Fogassi, L. (2003) Action recognition in the primate motor cortex can be a basis for communication. Paper presented at the conference, Vocalize to localize: A missing piece in the puzzling route towards language. Grenoble, France, January 2003. [PFM]Google Scholar
Frangiskakis, J. M., Ewart, A. K., Morris, C. A., Mervis, C. B., Bertrand, J., Robinson, B. F., Klein, B. P., Ensing, G. J., Everett, L. A., Green, E. D., Proschel, C., Gutowski, N. J., Noble, M., Atkinson, D. L., Odelberg, S .J. & Keating, M. T. (1996) Lim-kinase 1 hemizygosity implicated in impaired visuospatial constructive cognition. Cell 86:5969. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H. & Fahle, M. (2000) Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science 11(1):2025. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Freyd, J. J. (1987) Dynamic mental representation. Psychological Review 94:427–38. [IB]Google Scholar
Galati, G., Lobel, E., Vallar, G., Berthoz, A., Pizzamiglio, L. & Bihan, D. L. (2000) The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: A functional magnetic resonance study. Experimental Brain Research 133:156–64. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Galletti, C. & Battaglini, P. P. (1989) Gaze-dependent visual neurons in area V3A of monkey prestriate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 9:1112–25. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gaymard, B., Siegler, I., Rivaud-Pechoux, S., Israel, I., Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. & Berthoz, A. (2000) A common mechanism for the control of eye and head movements in humans. Annals of Neurology 47(6):819–22. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B. & Mangun, G. R. (2002) Cognitive neuroscience. Norton. [PFM]Google Scholar
Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. R. (1978) The child's understanding of number. Academic Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gentilucci, M., Scandolara, C. Pigarev, I. N. & Rizzolatti, G. (1983) Visual responses in the postarcuate cortex (area 6) of the monkey that are independent of eye position. Experimental Brain Research 50:464–68. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gerardin, E., Sirigu, A., Lehericy, S., Poline, J. P., Gaymard, B., Marsault, C., Agid, Y. & Le Bihan, D. (2000) Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cerebral Cortex 10:10931104. [BW]Google Scholar
Ghadakpour, L. (2003) Le systeme conceptuel, a l’interface entre le langage, le raisonnement et l’espace qualitatif: Vers un modele de representations ephemeres. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation.) Ecole Polytechnique, Paris. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Gibson, B. S. (1994) Visual attention and objects: One versus two or convex versus concave? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20(1):203207. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J. & Spelke, E. S. (1983) The development of perception. In: Handbook of child psychology: Cognitive development, vol. 3, ed. Mussen, P., Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. Jossey Bass. [SL]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. (I979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin. [GG]Google Scholar
Gillett, G. (1992) Representation, meaning and thought. Clarendon. [GG]Google Scholar
Givon, T. (1995) Functionalism and grammar. Benjamins. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S. & Houle, S. (1998) Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10:293302. [BW]Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995) Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. University of Chicago Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Goldberg, G. (1985) Supplementary motor area structure and function: Review and hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:567616. [PFM]Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A., Meenan, J. P., Bülthoff, H. H., Nicolle, D. A., Murphy, K. J. & Racicot, C. I. (1994) Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension. Current Biology 4(7):604–10. [MLA, IB, aJRH]Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A. & Milner, D. (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences 15(1):2025. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, J. & Goldberg, M. (1998) The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature 391(29):481–84. [IB]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Eldredge, N. (1977) Punctuated equilibria: The tempo and mode of evolution reconsidered. Paleobiology 3:115–51. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Graziano, M. S. A., Hu, X. T. A. & Gross, C. G. (1997) Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 77(5):2268–92. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Graziano, M. S. A., Reiss, L. A. J. & Gross, C. G. (1999) A neuronal representation of the location of nearby sounds. Nature 397(6718):428–30. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Gross, C. G. (1992) Representation of visual stimuli in inferior temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B - Biological Sciences 335(1273):310. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G. & Blake, R. (2000) Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12(5):711–20. [AK]Google Scholar
Haack, S. (1994) Deviant logics. In: The encyclopedia of language and linguistics, ed. Asher, R. E. & Simpson, J. M. Y. Pergamon Press. [rJRH]Google Scholar
Hale, K. & Keyser, S. J. (2002) Prolegomenon to a theory of argument structure. MIT Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Haxby, J. V., Grady, C. L., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, M., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, P., Schapiro, M. B. & Rapoport, S. I. (1991) Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 88:1621–25. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hendry, S. H. C. & Yoshioka, T. (1994) A neurochemically distinct third channel in the macaque dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Science 264:575–77. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Heywood, C. A. & Kentridge, R. W (2000) Affective blindsight? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(4):125–26. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Higginbotham, J. T. (1985) On semantics. L.I. 16(4):547–93. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Higginbotham, J. T. (1989) Elucidations of meaning. Linguistic and Philosophy 12(3):465517. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E., Plaut, D. C. & Shallice, T. (1993) Simulating brain damage. Scientific American, 269(4):5865. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1958) A course in modern linguistics. Macmillan. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Holland, J. H. (1992) Adaptation in natural and artificial systems. MIT Press. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1968) Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology 195:215–43. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1740/1978) A treatise of human nature. Clarendon Press. [FC]Google Scholar
Hummel, J. (1999) Binding problem. In: The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences, ed. Wilson, R. A. & Keil, F. C. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (1987) Language and number: The emergence of a cognitive system. Basil Blackwell. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (1999) Individuals are abstractions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(4):620–21. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2000a) Languages treat 1–4 specially. Mind and Language 16(1):6975. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2000b) Social transmission favours linguistic generalization. In: The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Hurford, J. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2001) Protothought had no logical names. In: New essays on the origin of language, ed. Trabant, J. & Ward, S. De Gruyter. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2002) The roles of expression and representation in language evolution. In: The transition to language, ed. Wray, A. Oxford University Press. [aJRH, AK]Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press. [IB]Google Scholar
Indurkhya, B. (1992) Metaphor and cognition. Kluwer Academic. [BI]Google Scholar
Indurkhya, B. (1998) On creation of features and change of representation. Journal of Japanese Cognitive Science Society 5(2):4356. [BI]Google Scholar
Ingle, D. J. (1973) Two visual systems in the frog. Science 181:1053–55. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ingle, D. J. (1980) Some effects of pretectum lesions on the frog's detection of stationary objects. Behavioral Brain Research 1:139–63. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ingle, D. J. (1982) Organization of visuomotor behaviors in vertebrates. In: Analysis of visual behavior, ed. Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A., & Mansfield, R. J. W. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2002) Foundations of language. Oxford University Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. & Landau, B. (1992) Spatial language and spatial cognition. In: Languages of the mind: Essays on mental representation, ed. Jackendoff, R. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G. & Sakata, H. (1995) Grasping objects – the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences 18:(7)314–20. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Johansson, G. (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics 14:201–11. [AK]Google Scholar
Johnson, A. (2003) Families of the forest: The Matsigenka Indians of the Peruvian Amazon. University of California Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Steedman, M. (1978) The psychology of syllogisms. Cognitive Psychology 10:6499. [GP]Google Scholar
Johnsrude, I. S., Owen, A. M., Crane, J., Milner, B. & Evans, A. C. (1999) A cognitive activation study of memory for spatial relationships. Neuropsychologia 37:829–41. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Jones, G. V. (1985) Deep dyslexia, imageability, and ease of predication. Brain and Language 24:119. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Jones, G. V. (2002) Predicability (ease of predication) as semantic substrate of imageability in reading and retrieval. Brain and Language 82:159–66. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Kaas, J. H. & Hackett, T. A. (1999) “What” and “where” processing in auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience 2(12):1045–47. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Treisman., A. (1984) Changing views of attention and automaticity. In Varieties of attention, ed. Parasuraman, R. & Davies, D. A. Academic Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Treisman., A. (1992) The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology 24:175219. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kamp, H. (1981) A theory of truth and semantic representation. In: Formal methods in the study of language: Proceedings of the Third Amsterdam Colloquium, Mathematical Centre Tracts, pp. 277–322, ed. Groenendijk, J., Janssen, T. M. V. & Stokhof, M. [Reprinted in: Truth, interpretation and information (1984), ed. Groenendijk, J., Janssen, T. M. V. & Stokhof, M..] Foris. [rJRH]Google Scholar
Kamp, H. & Partee, B. (1995) Prototype theory and compositionality. Cognition, 57(2):129–91. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Kamp, H. & Reyle, U. (1993) From discourse to logic: Introduction to modeltheoretic semantics of natural language, formal logic and discourse representation theory. Kluwer Academic. [arJRH]Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1783) Prolegomena zu einer jeden künftigen Metaphysik die als Wissenschaft wird auftreten können. Johann Friedrich Hartknoch. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1789/1929) Critique of pure reason, trans. Kemp-Smith, N. Macmillan. [GG]Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1905) Prolegomena to any future metaphysics, trans. Carus, P. Open Court. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Keenan, E. (1987) Facing the truth. In: Universal grammar: Fifteen essays, ed. Keenan, E. Croom Helm. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kikuchi-Yorioka, Y. & Sawaguchi, T. (2000) Parallel visuospatial and audiospatial working memory processes in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 3(11):1075–76. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kirby, S. (1999) Syntax out of learning: The cultural evolution of structured communication in a population of induction algorithms. In: Advances in artificial life, ed. Floreano, D., Nicoud, J. D. & Mondada, F.. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, No. 1674. Springer. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kirby, S. (2000) Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In: The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Hurford, J. R. Cambridge University Press. [J-LD, aJRH]Google Scholar
Korte, M. & Rauschecker, J. P. (1993) Auditory spatial tuning of cortical neurons is sharpened in cats with early blindness. Journal of Neurophysiology 70(4):1717–21. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L. & Alpert, N. M. (1998) Mental rotation of objects versus hands: Neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology 35:151–61. [BW]Google Scholar
Kotovsky, A. & Baillargeon, R. (1998) The development of calibration-based reasoning about collision events in young infants. Cognition 67:311–51. [PFD]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Dawkins, R. (1984) Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In: Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. Blackwell Scientific. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1980) Naming and necessity. Blackwell. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Landau, B. & Jackendoff, R. (1993) “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(2):217–38. [BB, arJRH]Google Scholar
Landau, B, Smith, L. B. & Jones, S. S. (1988) The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development 3:299321. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Landau, B, Smith, L. B. & Jones, S. S. (1998a) Object perception and object naming in early development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(1):1924. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Landau, B, Smith, L. B. & Jones, S. S. (1998b) Object shape, object function and object name. Journal of Memory and Language 38:127. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Langer, S. K. (1957) Philosophy in a new key, 3rd edition. Harvard University Press. [BI]Google Scholar
Lehky, S. R. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1988) Network model of shape-from-shading – neural function arises from both receptive and projective fields. Nature 333:452–54. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S. & Pitts, W. H. (1959) What the frog's eye tells the frog's brain. Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers 47:1940–51. [Reprinted in McCulloch 1988]. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Liddell, S. K. (1990) Four functions of a locus: Re-examining the structure of a space in ASL. In: Sign language research: Theoretical issues, ed. Lucas, C. Gallaudet University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Livingstone, M. & Hubel, D. (1988) Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology and perception. Science 240:740–69. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1689/1975) An essay concerning human understanding. Clarendon Press. [FC]Google Scholar
Luck, S. J. & Vogel, E. K. (1997) The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390:279–81. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973) The working brain. Penguin. [GG]Google Scholar
Luschei, E. S. & Goldberg, L. J. (1981) Neural mechanisms of mandibular control: Mastication and voluntary biting. In: Handbook of physiology: The nervous system, vol. 2, ed. Brookhart, J. M. American Physiological Society. [PFM]Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977) Semantics (2 vols.). Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (1998) The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:499546. [PFM]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (2000) On the origin of internal structure of word forms. Science 288:527–31. [PFM]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (2001) Motor mechanisms in speech ontogeny: Phylogenetic, neurobiological and linguistic implications. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11:696700. [PFM]Google Scholar
MacSweeney, M., Woll, B., Campbell, R., Calvert, G. A., McGuire, P. K., David, A. S., Simmons, A. & Brammer, M. J. (2002) Neural correlates of British Sign Language processing: Specific regions for topographic language? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(7):1064–75. [BW]Google Scholar
Mandler, G. & Shebo, B. J (1982) Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111:122. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1998) Blindsight and shape perception: Deficit of visual consciousness or of visual function? Brain 121:1565–88. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Marois, R., Chung Leung, H. & Gore, J. (2000) A stimulus-driven approach to object identity and location processing in the human brain. Neuron 25:717–28. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L, Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. (1996) Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature 379:649–52. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Maye, A. (2002) Neuronale Synchronität, zeitliche Bindung und Wahrnehmung. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). TU. [MW]Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982) The growth of biological thought. Belknap. [PFM]Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1968) Programs with common sense. In: Semantic information processing, ed. Minsky, M. L. MIT Press. [TJA]Google Scholar
McCulloch, W. S. (1988) Embodiments of mind. MIT Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
McCulloch, W. S. & Pitts, W. H. (1943) A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5:115–33. [TJA]Google Scholar
McDonald, B. H. (1994) Sign language morphology. In: The encyclopedia of language and linguistics, ed. Asher, R. E. & Simpson, J. M. Y. Pergamon Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Meredith, M. A. & Stein, B. E. (1983) Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221:389–91. [TJA]Google Scholar
Meredith, M. A. & Stein, B. E. (1986) Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. Journal of Neurophysiology 56:640–22. [TJA]Google Scholar
Merigan, W. H, Byrne, C. E. & Maunsell, J. H. R. (1991) Does primate motion perception depend on the magnocellular pathway? The Journal of Neuroscience 11(11):3422–29. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Merigan, W. H. & Maunsell, J. H. R. (1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience 16:369402. [aJRH, MW]Google Scholar
Michalski, R. S. & Stepp, R. E. (1983) Learning from observation: Conceptual clustering. In: Machine learning – An artificial approach. Tioga. [J-LD]Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. (1946/1963) The perception of causality, trans. Miles, T. R. & Mile, E. Basic Books. [SL]Google Scholar
Milner, A. D. (1998) Streams and consciousness: Visual awareness and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(1):2530. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Milner, A. D. & Goodale, M. A. (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press. [BB, aJRH, PFM]Google Scholar
Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G. & Macko, K. A. (1983) Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends in Neuroscience 6:414–17. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Mithun, M. (2000) Noun and verb in Iroquoian languages: Multicategorisation from multiple criteria. In: Approaches to the typology of word classes, ed. Vogel, P. M. & Comrie, B. Mouton de Gruyter. [MW]Google Scholar
Montague, R. (1970) English as a formal language. In: Linguaggi nella Società e nella Tecnica, ed. Visentini, B. Edizioni di Comunità. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Montague, R. (1973) The proper treatment of quantification in ordinary English. In: Approaches to natural language, ed. Hintikka, J., Moravcsik, J. & Suppes, P. Reidel. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Moore, T., Rodman, H. R. & Gross, C. G. (1998) Man, monkey, and blindsight. Neuroscientist 4(4):227–30. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Morris, C. W. (1938) Foundations of the theory of signs. Chicago University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Morris, C. W. (1946) Signs, language, and behavior. Prentice-Hall. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Morris, J. S, Ohman, A. & Dolan, R. J. (1999) A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 96(4):1680–85. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1960) Social structure. Macmillan. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Nakamura, K. (1999) Auditory spatial discriminatory and mnemonic neurons in rat posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 82(5):2503–17. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1967) Cognitive psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1982) The knowledge level. Artificial Intelligence 18:82127. [TJA]Google Scholar
Newman, E. A. & Hartline, P. H. (1981) Integration of visual and infrared information in bimodal neurons of the rattlesnake optic tectum. Science 213:789–91. [TJA]Google Scholar
Nowak, M. A., Plotkin, J. B & Jansen, V. A. A. (2000) The evolution of syntactic communication. Nature 404:495–98. [aJRH]Google Scholar
O’Brien, G. & Opie, J. (1999) A connectionist theory of phenomenal experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(1):127–48. [aJRH]Google Scholar
O’Craven, K., Downing, P. & Kanwisher, N. (1999) fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature 401(7):584–87. [IB]Google Scholar
Padden, C. (1990) The relation between space and grammar in ASL verb morphology. In: Sign language research: Theoretical issues, ed. Lucas, C. Gallaudet University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1971) Imagery and verbal processes. Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1983) The empirical case for dual coding. In: Imagery, memory and cognition, ed. Yuille, J. C. Erlbaum. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Parsons, T. (1990) Events in the semantics of English: A study in subatomic semantics. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Patton, P. E. & Anastasio, T. J. (2003) Modeling cross-modal enhancement and modality-specific suppression in multisensory neurons. Neural Computation 15:783810. [TJA]Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. (1999) Can a parrot respond to absence of information? In: The Alex studies, ed. Pepperberg, I. Harvard University Press. [rJRH]Google Scholar
Perrett, D. I., Harries, M. H., Bevan, R., Thomas, S., Benson, P. J., Mistlin, A. J., Chitty, A. J., Hietanen, J. K. & Ortega, J. E. (1989) Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. Journal of Experimental Biology 146:87113. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953) The origin of intelligence in the child, trans. Cook, M. Routledge. (Also published Penguin, 1977.) [BI]Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962) Play, dreams and imitation in childhood, trans. Gattegno, C. & Hodgson, F. M. Norton. [BI]Google Scholar
Pianesi, F. & Varzi, A. C. (2000) Events and event talk. In: Speaking of events, ed. Higginbotham, J., Pianesi, F. & Varzi, A. C. Oxford University Press. [MW]Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C. (1999) A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: Extension to sequential processing. Cognitive Science 23:543–68. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C. & Shallice, T. (1993) Deep dyslexia: A case study of connectionist neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology 10:377500. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, J. (1995) The generative lexicon. MIT Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1989) The role of location indexes in spatial perception: A sketch of the FINST spatial-index model. Cognition 32:6597. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1999) Is vision continuous with cognition? – The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(3):341423. [IB]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2000) Situating vision in the world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(5):197207. [aJRH, IB]Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. (1961) On what there is. In: Quine, M. V. (1961) From a logical point of view, 2nd edition. Harvard University Press. [MW]Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. & Blakeslee, S. (1998) Phantoms in the brain: Human nature and the architecture of the mind. Fourth Estate. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Rauschecker, J. P. (1997) Processing of complex sounds in the auditory cortex of cat, monkey, and man. Acta Otolaryngolica Supplement 532:3438. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Redican, W. K. (1975) Facial expressions in nonhuman primates. In: Primate behavior: Developments in field and laboratory research, vol. 4, ed. Rosenblum, L. A. Academic Press. [PFM]Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D., Carpenter, P. & Just, M. A. (2000) The neural bases of strategy and skill in sentence-picture verification. Cognitive Psychology 40:261–95. [BW]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience 21(5):188–94. [MAA, BB, PFD, aJRH, PFM]Google Scholar
Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S. & Rauschecker, J. P. (1999) Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 2(12):1131–36. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Russac, R. J. (1983) Early discrimination among small object collections. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 36:124–38. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1905) On denoting. Mind 14:479–93. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1912 /1998) The problems of philosophy. Oxford University Press. [FC]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1918 /1985) The philosophy of logical atomism. Open Court. [FC]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1940) An inquiry into meaning and truth. Allen and Unwin. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1948) Human knowledge, its scope and limits. Allen and Unwin. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1957) Mr. Strawson on referring. Mind 66:385–89. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1959) My philosophical development. Allen and Unwin. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1957) The theory of meaning. In: British philosophy in the mid-century, ed. Mace, C. A. Allen and Unwin. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Sanders, M. D., Warrington, E. K., Marshall, J. & Weiskrantz, L. (1974) “Blindsight”: Vision in a field defect. Lancet 20:707708. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Schaeffer, B., Eggleston, V. H. & Scott, J. L. (1974) Number development in young children. Cognitive Psychology 6:357–79. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Schein, B. (1993) Plurals and events. MIT Press. [MP-P]Google Scholar
Schillen, T. B. & König, P. (1994) Binding by temporal structure in multiple feature domains of an oscillatory neuronal network. Biological Cybernetics 70:397405. [MW]Google Scholar
Scholl, B. J. & Nakayama, K. (2002) Causal capture: Contextual effects on the perception of collision events. Psychological Science 13:493–98. [SL]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1979) Expression and meaning: Studies in the theory of speech acts. Cambridge University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Singer, W. & Gray, C. M. (1995) Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18:555–86. [MW]Google Scholar
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S. & Landau, B. (1996) Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition 60:143171. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Snell, W. (1964) Kinship relations in Machiguenga. Master's thesis, Hartford Seminary. Hartford, CT. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Stanner, W. E. H. (1937) Aboriginal modes of address and reference in the northwest of the Northern Territory. Oceania 7:300–15. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Starkey, P. & Cooper, R. G. (1980) Perception of numbers by human infants. Science 210:1033–35. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Steels, L. (1997) Constructing and sharing perceptual distinctions. Coupling of meaning creation and naming games in software experiments. In: Proceedings of the European conference on machine learning, ed. van Someren, M. & Widmer, G. Springer. [MLA]Google Scholar
Stein, B. E. & Meredith, M. A. (1993) The merging of the senses. MIT Press. [TJA]Google Scholar
Stein, B. E. & Wallace, M. T. (1996) Comparisons of cross-modality integration in midbrain and cortex. Progress in Brain Research 112:289–99. [TJA]Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1950) On referring. Mind 59:320–44. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1959) Subject and predicate in logic and grammar. Methuen. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1974) Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. Methuen. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Stricanne, B., Andersen, R. A& Mazzoni, P. (1996) Eye-centered, head-centered, and intermediate coding of remembered sound locations in area LIP. Journal of Neurophysiology 76(3):2071–76. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (1975) Semantics and syntax of motion. In: Syntax and semantics, vol.4, ed. Kimball, J. P. Academic Press. [SL]Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000) Toward a cognitive semantics. MIT Press. [rJRH, SL]Google Scholar
Thornton, I., Rensink, R. & Shiffrar, M. (2002) Active vs. passive processing of biological motion. Perception 31:837–53. [AK]Google Scholar
Tian, B. & Rauschecker, J. P. (1998) Processing of frequency-modulated sounds in the cat's posterior auditory field. Journal of Neurophysiology 79(5):2629–42. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. B. (1968) Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung 31:299337. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. B. (2001) The neurobiology of early communication: Intersubjective regulations in human brain development. Handbook of Brain and Behaviour in Human Development 841–81. [GG]Google Scholar
Trick, L. M. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993) What enumeration studies tell us about spatial attention: Evidence for limited capacity pre-attentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Human Perception and Performance 19(2):331–51. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Trick, L. M. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1994) Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited capacity pre-attentive stage in vision. Psychological Review 10:123. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1984) Visual routines. Cognition 18:97157. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G., Courtney, S. M. & Haxby, J. V. (1998) A neural system for human visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95:883–90. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G. & Mishkin, M. (1982) Two cortical visual systems. In: Analysis of visual behavior, ed. Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A., & Mansfield, R. J. W. MIT Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P. & Belliveau, J. W. (2001) Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98(20):11656–61. [rJRH]Google Scholar
Valdes-Sosa, M., Cobo, A. & Pinilla, T. (1998) Transparent motion and objectbased attention. Cognition 66:B13B23. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Valiant, L. G. (1994) Circuits of the mind. Oxford University Press. [TJA]Google Scholar
Van Essen, D. C, Anderson, C. H. & Felleman, D. J. (1992) Information processing in the primate visual system: An integrated systems perspective. Science 255:419–23. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Vecera, S. P. & Farah, M. J. (1994) Does visual attention select objects or locations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 123(2):146–60. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Walker, S. (1983) Animal thought. Routledge. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Weeks, R., Horwitz, B., Aziz-Sultan, A., Tian, B., Wessinger, C. M., Cohen, L. G., Hallett, M. & Rauschecker, J. P. (2000) A positron emission tomographic study of auditory localization in the congenitally blind. Journal of Neuroscience 20(7):2664–72. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Wegener, P. (1885/1971) The life of speech. In: Speech and reason, ed. Wegener, P. & Abse, D. W. University of Virginia Press. [BI]Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. (1986) Blindsight: A case study and implications. Oxford University Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. (1997) Consciousness lost and found. Oxford University Press. [aJRH, IB]Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E., Sanders, M. & Marshall, J. (1974) Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain 97:709–28. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Werning, M. (2001) How to solve the problem of compositionality by oscillatory networks. In: Proceedings of the twenty-third annual conference of the cognitive science society, ed. Moore, J. D. & Stenning, K. Erlbaum. [MW]Google Scholar
Werning, M. (2003a) Synchrony and composition: Toward a cognitive architecture between classicism and connectionism. In: Applications of mathematical logic in philosophy and linguistics, ed. Loewe, B., Malzkorn, W. & Raesch, T. Kluwer. [MW]Google Scholar
Werning, M. (2003b) The temporal dimension of thought: Cortical foundations of predicative representation. Synthese. (in press). [MW]Google Scholar
Westwood, D. A., Chapman, C. D. & Roy, E. A. (2000) Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream. Experimental Brain Research 130:545–48. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Healy, H. G. & Ellis, N. C. (1999) The effect of imageability and predicability of cues in autobiographical memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 52A:555–79. [GVJ]Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1922) Tractatus logico-philosophicus. Routledge. [FC]Google Scholar
Woll, B. & Kyle, J. (1994) Sign language. In: The encyclopedia of language and linguistics, ed. Asher, R. E. & Simpson, J. M. Y. Pergamon Press. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Wray, A. (1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language and Communication 18:4767. [BI]Google Scholar
Wray, A. (2000) A protolanguage with no declaratives and no names. Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on the Evolution of Language, Paris. [BI]Google Scholar
Wray, A. & Perkins, M. R. (2000) The functions of formulaic language: An integrated model. Language and Communication 20:128. [BI]Google Scholar
Wurtz, R. H. & Kandel, E. R. (2000) Perception of motion, depth, and form. In: Principles of neural science, ed. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H. & Jessell, T. M. McGraw-Hill. [MW]Google Scholar
Yantis, S. (1998) Control of visual attention. In: Attention, ed. Pashler, H. Psychology Press. [IB]Google Scholar
Yantis, S. & Johnson, D. N. (1990) Mechanisms of attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:812–25. [IB]Google Scholar
Zacks, J. M., Braver, T. S., Sheridan, M. A., Donaldson, D. I., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Buckner, R. L. & Raichle, M. E. (2001a) Human brain activity time-locked to perceptual event boundaries. Nature Neuroscience 4:651–55. [SL]Google Scholar
Zacks, J. M. & Tversky, B. (2001) Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin 127:321. [SL]Google Scholar
Zacks, J. M., Tversky, B. & Iyer, G. (2001b) Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130:2958. [SL]Google Scholar
Zadeh, L. A. & Kacprzyk, J., eds. (1992) Fuzzy logic for the management of uncertainty. Wiley. [rJRH]Google Scholar
Zeki, S. (1993) A vision of the brain. Blackwell Scientific. [aJRH]Google Scholar
Zhuo, Y., Zhou, T. G., Rao, H. Y., Wang, J. J., Meng, M., Chen, M., Zhou, C. & Chen, L. (2003) Contributions of the visual ventral pathway to long-range apparent motion. Science 299(5605):417–20. [rJRH]Google Scholar