Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T20:51:07.564Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

THE CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF GRAMMATICALITY JUDGMENT TESTS AS MEASURES OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT KNOWLEDGE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 May 2013

Xavier Gutiérrez*
Affiliation:
University of Windsor
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xavier Gutiérrez, Department of Languages, Literatures, and Cultures, University of Windsor, 401 Sunset Avenue, N9B 3P4, Windsor, Ontario, Canada. E-mail: xavierg@uwindsor.ca

Abstract

Grammaticality judgment tests (GJTs) have been, and continue to be, frequently used in the field of SLA as a measure of learners’ linguistic ability in the second language (L2). However, only a few studies have examined their construct validity as measures of implicit and explicit knowledge (Bowles, 2011; R. Ellis, 2005), and even fewer have explored in detail how features of these tests, such as time pressure and task stimulus, affect their construct validity (Loewen, 2009). The purpose of this paper is to examine the effect that time pressure and task stimulus have on the type of knowledge representations on which L2 learners draw when performing GJTs. The results show that the grammatical and ungrammatical sections of a timed and an untimed GJT loaded differently in both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. This finding can be interpreted as indicating that grammatical and ungrammatical sentences constitute measures of implicit and explicit knowledge, respectively. Additionally, the results show that time pressure and task stimulus have significant effects on learners’ performance on GJTs.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Arbuckle, J. (2010). AMOS (Version 19.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago: SPSS.Google Scholar
Arrindell, W. A., & van der Ende, J. (1985). An empirical test of the utility of the observations-to-variables ratio in factor and components analysis. Applied Psychological Measurement, 9, 165178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bard, E. G., Robertson, D., & Sorace, A. (1996). Magnitude estimation of linguistic acceptability. Language, 72, 3268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, P. T., & Kline, P. (1981). The observation to variable ratio in factor analysis. Personality Study in Group Behavior, 1, 2333.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1979). Explicit and implicit judgments of L2 grammaticality. Language Learning, 29, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development, 57, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D. (1989). Metalinguistic performance and interlinguistic competence. New York: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R., Felix, S., & Ioup, G. (1988). The accessibility of Universal Grammar in adult language learning. Second Language Research, 4, 132.Google Scholar
Bowles, M. (2011). Measuring implicit and explicit linguistic knowledge: What can heritage language learners contribute? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 247271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1978). The scientific use of factor analysis. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, W. D., & Kaplan, T. (1998). Native speaker vs. L2 learner grammaticality judgments. Applied Linguistics, 19, 183203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jong, N. (2005). Can second language grammar be learned through listening? An experimental study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 205234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning. In Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313348). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2009). Cognitive-psychological processes in second language learning. In Long, M. H. & Doughty, C. J. (Eds.), The handbook of language teaching (pp. 119138). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, D. (2001). Performance consistency in second language acquisition and language testing: a conceptual gap. Second Language Research, 17, 442456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. (2011). Implicit and explicit SLA and their interface. In Sanz, C., & Leow, R. P. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit language learning: Conditions, processes, and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism (pp. 3547). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (1991). Grammaticality judgments and second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 161186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (1994). A theory of instructed second language acquisition. In Ellis, N. C. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 79114). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2004). The definition and measurement of L2 explicit knowledge. Language Learning, 54, 227275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2009). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. In Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 3164). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R., & Loewen, S. (2007). Confirming the operational definitions of explicit and implicit knowledge in Ellis (2005): Responding to Isemonger. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 119126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Gass, S. (1983). The development of L2 intuitions. TESOL Quarterly, 17, 273291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor analysis (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goss, N., Ying-Hua, Z., & Lantolf, J. (1994). Two heads may be better than one: Mental activity in second-language grammaticality judgments. In Tarone, E., Gass, S. M., & Cohen, A. (Eds.), Research methodology in second-language acquisition (pp. 263286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Han, Y. (2000). Grammaticality judgment tests: How reliable and valid are they? Applied Language Learning, 11, 177204.Google Scholar
Han, Y., & Ellis, R. (1998). Implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge and general language proficiency. Language Teaching Research, 2, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedgcock, J. (1993). Well-formed vs. ill-formed strings in L2 metalingual tasks: Specifying features of grammaticality judgments. Second Language Research, 9, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstjin, J. H. (2005). Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 129140.Google Scholar
Isemonger, I. M. (2007). Operational definitions of explicit and implicit knowledge: Response to R. Ellis (2005) and some recommendations for future research in this area. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 101118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. (2001). Psycholinguistically oriented second language research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 207222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 141151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and causality. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Klein, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kline, P. (1994). An easy guide to factor analysis. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Loewen, S. (2009). Grammaticality judgment tests and the measurement of implicit and explicit L2 knowledge. In Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 94112). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. (2006). Corrective feedback in the chatroom: An experimental study. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 19, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 4, 8499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandell, P. (1999). On the reliability of grammaticality judgment tests in second language acquisition research. Second Language Research, 15, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martens, M. J. (1988). Recognition and production of pronouns by francophone learners of English as a second language (Unpublished master’s thesis). Concordia University, Montreal.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2005). On knowledge and development of unaccusativity in Spanish L2. Linguistics, 43, 11531190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2011). Morphological errors in Spanish second language learners and heritage speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 163192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulaik, S. A. (2009). Foundations of factor analysis (2nd ed.) Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennell, R. (1968). The influence of communality and N on the sampling distributions of factor loadings. Psychometrika, 33, 423439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (1985). Metalinguistic knowledge and language use in acquisition-poor environments. Applied Linguistics, 6, 239254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toth, P. D. (2006). Processing instruction and a role for output in second language acquisition. Language Learning, 56, 319385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A. (2005). Theoretical and methodological perspectives on the use of grammaticality judgment tasks in linguistic theory. Second Language Studies, 24, 129167.Google Scholar
Velicer, W. F., & Fava, J. L. (1998). Effects of variable and subject sampling on factor pattern recovery. Psychological Methods, 3, 231251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velicer, W. F., Peacock, A. C., & Jackson, D. N. (1982). A comparison of component and factor patterns: A Monte Carlo approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 17, 371388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warner, R. M. (2008). Applied statistics: From bivariate through multivariate techniques. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar