Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T14:38:13.082Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

WHEN GENDER AND LOOKING GO HAND IN HAND

Grammatical Gender Processing In L2 Spanish

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 June 2013

Paola E. Dussias*
Affiliation:
Penn State University, Center for Language Science
Jorge R. Valdés Kroff
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Rosa E. Guzzardo Tamargo
Affiliation:
Universidad de Puerto Rico
Chip Gerfen
Affiliation:
American University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paola E. Dussias, 237 Burrowes Building, Department of Spanish, Italian & Portuguese, Penn State University, University Park, PA 16802; E-mail: pdussias@psu.edu

Abstract

In a recent study, Lew-Williams and Fernald (2007) showed that native Spanish speakers use grammatical gender information encoded in Spanish articles to facilitate the processing of upcoming nouns. In this article, we report the results of a study investigating whether grammatical gender facilitates noun recognition during second language (L2) processing. Sixteen monolingual Spanish participants (control group) and 18 English-speaking learners of Spanish (evenly divided into high and low Spanish proficiency) saw two-picture visual scenes in which items matched or did not match in gender. Participants’ eye movements were recorded while they listened to 28 sentences in which masculine and feminine target items were preceded by an article that agreed in gender with the two pictures or agreed only with one of the pictures. An additional group of 15 Italian learners of Spanish was tested to examine whether the presence of gender in the first language (L1) modulates the degree to which gender is used during L2 processing. Data were analyzed by comparing the proportion of eye fixations on the objects in each condition. Monolingual Spanish speakers looked sooner at the referent on different-gender trials than on same-gender trials, replicating results reported in past literature. Italian-Spanish bilinguals exhibited a gender anticipatory effect, but only for the feminine condition. For the masculine condition, participants waited to hear the noun before identifying the referent. Like the Spanish monolinguals, the highly proficient English-Spanish speakers showed evidence of using gender information during online processing, whereas the less proficient learners did not. The results suggest that both proficiency in the L2 and similarities between the L1 and the L2 modulate the usefulness of morphosyntactic information during speech processing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The writing of this article was supported in part by the National Science Foundation (NSF) grant BCS-0821924 to Paola E. Dussias and Chip Gerfen, by NSF grants BCS-0955090 and OISE-0968369 to Judith F. Kroll and Paola E. Dussias, and by a National Science Foundation graduate research fellowship to Jorge Valdés Kroff. We would like to thank the audience at Penn State’s Center for Language Science and the audience at the 2010 Second Language Research Forum for helpful comments. All errors are our own.

References

Ainsworth-Darnell, K., Shulman, H. G., & Boland, J. E. (1998). Dissociating brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 112130.Google Scholar
Alameda, J. R., & Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de las unidades lingüísticas del castellano: Volumen I: Orden alfabético / Volumen II: Orden por frecuencias [Dictionary of the linguistic units of Spanish: Volume I: Ordered alphabetically / Volume II: Ordered by frequencies]. Oviedo, Spain: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Oviedo.Google Scholar
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247264.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 137153.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernández, A., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1996). Gender priming in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 85, 9921004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5, 341345.Google Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L. (2002). L2 acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In Pérez-Leroux, A. T. & Liceras, J. M. (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax: The L1/L2 connection (pp. 151176). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Carello, C., Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Rapid naming is affected by association but not by syntax. Memory & Cognition, 16, 187195.Google Scholar
Carroll, S. (1989). Second-language acquisition and the computational paradigm. Language Learning, 39, 535594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colé, P., & Segui, J. (1994). Grammatical incongruency and vocabulary types. Memory & Cognition, 22, 387394.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. (1974). The control of eye-fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Kovacic, D., Fedorenko, E., & Caramazza, A. (2003). The gender congruency effect and the selection of free-standing and bound morphemes: Evidence from Croatian. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29, 12701282.Google Scholar
Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M., & Job, R. (2005). Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 4259.Google Scholar
Cudeck, R., & Klebe, K. J. (2002). Multiphase mixed-effects models for repeated measures data. Psychological Methods, 7, 4163.Google Scholar
Dahan, D., Swingley, D., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S. (2000). Linguistic gender and spoken-word recognition in French. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 465480.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In Kroll, J. F. & de Groot, A. M. B. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 179201). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dowens, M. G., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 18701887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffield, N., & White, L. (1999). Assessing L2 knowledge of Spanish clitic placement: Converging methodologies. Second Language Research, 15, 133160.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. D., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G. (1996). Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 677758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faussart, C., Jakubowicz, C., & Costes, M. (1999). Gender and number processing in spoken French and Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 11, 75101.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speech in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-García, M. (1999). Patterns of gender agreement in the speech of second language learners. In Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. & Martínez-Gil, F. (Eds.), Advances in Hispanic linguistics: Papers from the 2nd Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 315). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, B. (1992). Saccadic reaction time: Implications for reading, dyslexia and visual cognition. In Rayner, K. (Ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading (pp. 3145). New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Franceschina, F. (2001). Against an L2 morphological deficit as an explanation for the differences between native and non-native grammars. In Foster-Cohen, S. H. & Nizegorodcew, A. (Eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook: Vol. 1 (pp. 143158). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Franceschina, F. (2005). Fossilized second language grammars: The acquisition of grammatical gender. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 467484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1-L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism, 14, 379399.Google Scholar
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2012). Can late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? Evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 226248.Google Scholar
Gess, R., & Herschensohn, J. (2001). Shifting the DP parameter: A study of Anglophone French L2ers. In Camps, J. & Wiltshire, C. R. (Eds.), Romance syntax, semantics and their L2 acquisition (pp. 105119). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gouvea, A. C., Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Poeppel, D. (2010). The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 149188.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F., Dommergues, J.-Y., Cornu, E., Guillelmon, D., & Besson, C. (1994). The gender-marking effect in spoken word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 590598.Google Scholar
Gurjanov, M., Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., Savić, M., & Turvey, M. T. (1985). Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by the gender of possessive adjectives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 692701.Google Scholar
Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 715728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y. (1997). The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The “failed functional features hypothesis.Second Language Research, 13, 187226.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Franceschina, F. (2004). Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In Prévost, P. & Paradis, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts (pp. 175205). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Kotz, S. A., Hofmann, J., Valentin, V. V., Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2004). The neural correlates of grammatical gender decisions in Spanish. Brain Imaging, 15, 863866.Google Scholar
Jacobsen, T. (1999). Effects of grammatical gender on picture and word naming: Evidence from German. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 499514.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., & Faussart, C. (1998). Gender agreement in the processing of spoken French. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 597617.Google Scholar
Jescheniak, J. D. (1999). Gender priming in picture naming: Modality and baseline effects. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 729737.Google Scholar
Kreysa, H., & Knoeferle, P. (2011). Effects of speaker gaze on spoken language comprehension: Task matters. In Carlson, L., Hölscher, C., & Shipley, T. F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 15571562). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 11261177.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18, 193198.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 447464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, C. (2007). Morphological variability in second language Spanish (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). McGill University, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morphosyntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 125142.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñan, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58, 503553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: An electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish-English bilinguals. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 205220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan-Short, K., Sanz, C., Steinhauer, K., & Ullman, M. T. (2010). Acquisition of gender agreement in second language learners: An event-related potential study. Language Learning, 60, 154193.Google Scholar
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785806.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., So, K.-F., Von Eckardt, B., & Feldman, L. B. (1984). Lexical and conceptual representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 2338.Google Scholar
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A. (2008). Second language processing: When are first and second languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 24, 397430.Google Scholar
Sabourin, L., Stowe, L. A., & de Haan, G. J. (2006). Transfer effects in learning a second language grammatical gender system. Second Language Research, 22, 129.Google Scholar
Saslow, M. G. (1967). Latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 10301033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiller, N. (2009, July). Grammatical gender in L2 from a processing perspective. Paper presented at the 7th International Symposium on Bilingualism, Utretch, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1986). Was weiss der Artikel vom Hauptwort? Ein Beitrag zur Verarbeitung syntaktischer Beziehungen beim Lesen [What does the article know about the noun? A study of syntactic agreement processing during reading]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 23, 150163.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1994). Word order and nominative case in non-native language acquisition: A longitudinal study of (L1 Turkish) German interlanguage. In Hoekstra, T. & Schwartz, B. D. (Eds.), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar (pp. 317368). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access model. Second Language Research, 12, 4072.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., & Spivey-Knowlton, M. J. (1996). Eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 583588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 286, 16321634.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2006). Eye movement and spoken language comprehension. In Traxler, M. J. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 835862). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73, 89134.Google Scholar
van Berkum, J. J. A. (1996). The psycholinguistics of grammatical gender. Nijmegen, Netherlands: Nijmegen University Press.Google Scholar
White, L. (1989). Linguistic universals, markedness and learnability: Comparing two different approaches. Second Language Research, 5, 127140.Google Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-MacGregor, M., & Leung, Y.-K. I. (2004). Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105133.Google Scholar
Wicha, N. Y. Y., Orozco-Figueroa, A., Reyes, I., Hernández, A., Gavaldón de Barreto, L., & Bates, E. (2005). When zebras become painted donkeys: Grammatical gender and semantic priming interact during picture integration in a spoken Spanish sentence. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 553587.Google Scholar