Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ph5wq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T08:10:57.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The use of psychological state words by late talkers at ages 3, 4, and 5 years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2008

ELIZA CARLSON LEE
Affiliation:
Bryn Mawr College
LESLIE RESCORLA*
Affiliation:
Bryn Mawr College
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Leslie Rescorla, Department of Psychology, 101 North Merion Avenue, Bryn Mawr, PA 19010. E-mail: lrescorl@brynmawr.edu

Abstract

The use of four types of psychological state words (physiological, emotional, desire, and cognitive) during mother–child play sessions at ages 3, 4, and 5 years was examined in 30 children diagnosed with delayed expressive language at 24–31 months and 15 age-matched comparison children with typical development. The children's mean length of utterance, total words uttered, lexical diversity, and use of propositional complements were assessed. The late talkers used significantly more physiological state words at ages 3 and 4, but the two groups did not differ in their use of physiological state terms at age 5. The late talkers used significantly fewer cognitive words than the comparison children at each age. The mothers of the late talkers made significantly fewer references to cognitive states than the mothers of the comparison children at each age. The delay in the emergence of cognitive state words in the preschool years may affect other aspects of late talkers’ cognitive and social development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children's talk about the mind. New York: Psychological Corporation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). The Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Specific language impairment as a maturational lag: Evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 442459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischel, J., Whitehurst, G., Caulfield, M., & DeBaryshe, B. (1989). Language growth in children with expressive language delay. Pediatrics, 82, 218227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, M. F. (1981). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Goffman, L., & Leonard, J. (2000). Growth of language skills in preschool children with specific language impairment: Implications for assessment and intervention. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 9, 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingshead, A. A. (1978). Four-Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. M., Terrell, S. L., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S., & Ross, H. S. (2003). A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in families. Child Development, 74, 905920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, J. R., & Kamhi, A. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 30, 6585.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R., Miller, J., & Tallal, P. (2001). Use of cognitive state predicates by language-impaired children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 349370.Google ScholarPubMed
Lee, E. C., & Rescorla, L. (2002). The use of psychological state terms by late talkers at age 3. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 623641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Miller, C., & Gerber, E. (1999). Grammatical morphology and the lexicon in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 678689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES language project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McKee, G., Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2000). Measuring vocabulary diversity using dedicated software. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 15, 323337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. J., Leonard, L. B. (2002). Lexical diversity in the spontaneous speech of children with specific language impairment: Application of D. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 927937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, R. (1996). Clinical implications of the natural history of slow expressive language development. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 5, 521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (2005). Age 13 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 459472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L., Alley, A., & Christine, J. B. (2001). Word frequencies in toddlers’ lexicons. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 598609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L., Dahlsgaard, K., & Roberts, J. (2000). Late-talking toddlers: MLU IPSyn outcomes at 3;0 and 4;0. Journal of Child Language, 27, 643664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Lee, E. C. (2000). Language impairment in young children. In Layton, T. L., Crais, E. R., & Watson, L. R. (Eds.), Handbook of early language impairment in children: Nature. Albany, NY: Delmar.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., Roberts, J., & Dahlsgaard, K. (1997). Late talkers at 2: Outcome at age 3. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 556566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L., & Schwartz, E. (1990). Outcome of toddlers with specific expressive language delay. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 393407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynell, J. K. (1977). Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Windsor, Canada: NFER.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between child and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Index of Productive Syntax. Applied Psychologinguistics, 11, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1990). Development of children with early language delay. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 7083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of first references to mental states. Cognition, 14, 301321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturn, A., & Johnston, J. (1999). Thinking out loud: An exploration of problem solving language in preschoolers with and without language-impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. (1988). Developmental language disorders. In Kavanaugh, J. F. & Truss, T. J. Jr. (Eds.), Learning disabilities: Proceedings of the National Conference (pp. 181272). Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Tardif, T., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology, 36, 2543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thal, D., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in late talkers: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, R. V., Kelly, D. J., Harbers, H. M., & Hollis, W. (1995). Measuring children's lexical diversity: Differentiating typical and impaired language learners. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 13491355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, R., Rice, M., & Moltz, C. (1993). Verb use by language-impaired and normally developing children. First Language, 37, 133143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windfuhr, K. L., Faragher, B., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2002). Lexical learning skills in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 415432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, E. C., & Perachio, J. J. (1983). Patterned Elicitation Syntax Test—Revised. Tucson, AZ: Communication Skill Builders.Google Scholar