Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-hgkh8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T01:45:11.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Commentary on Keynote

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2006

James Montgomery
Affiliation:
Ohio University
Julia Evans
Affiliation:
San Diego State University

Extract

In her Keynote, Gathercole (2006) provides a comprehensive review regarding the nature of the nonword repetition (NWR) task and a compelling argument for the utility of the task as a robust index of children's phonological short-term storage capacity. She further argues that temporary phonological storage acts as a primitive learning mechanism that plays an instrumental role in the lexical learning of young children, including children with specific language impairment (SLI). In this Commentary, we focus our remarks on two related themes concerning the relation of NWR and SLI: (a) performance on NWR tasks provides a window into these children's more general cognitive (dis)abilities and (b) the robustness of NWR as a predictor of these children's lexical knowledge and learning abilities.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bishop D., Bishop S., Bright P., Delaney T., & Tallal P. 1999. Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 155168.Google Scholar
Bishop D., North T., & Donlan C. 1996. Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391403.Google Scholar
Botting N., & Conti-Ramsden G. 2001. Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 421432.Google Scholar
Briscoe J., Bishop D., & Norbury C. 2001. Phonological processing, language, and literacy: A comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 320340.Google Scholar
Coady J., & Aslin R. 2004. Young children's sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 183213.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden G., Botting N., & Farragher B. 2001. Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741748.Google Scholar
Dollaghan C., Biber M., & Campbell T. 1993. Constituent syllable effects in a nonsense-word repetition task. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 10511054.Google Scholar
Dollaghan C., & Campbell T. 1998. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 11361146.Google Scholar
Edwards J., & Lahey M. 1998. Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 279309.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer S. 1996. Capacity limitations in working memory: The impact on lexical and morphological learning by children with language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 17, 3344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer S., Evans J., & Hesketh L. 1999. An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12491260.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer S., & Thordardottir E. 2002. Cognition and language. In P. Accardo, B. Rogers, & A. Capute (Eds.), Disorders of language development (pp. 2137). Timonium, MD: York Press.
Ellis Weismer S., Tomblin B., Zhang X., Buckwalter P., Chynoweth J., & Jones M. 2000. Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 865878.Google Scholar
Evans J., Viele Kass R., & Tang F. 2002. Grammatical morphology and perception of synthetic and natural speech in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 494504.Google Scholar
Gathercole S. E. 2006. Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship [Keynote]. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513543.Google Scholar
Gathercole S. E., & Baddeley A. 1990. Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.Google Scholar
Gray S. 2004. Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Predictors and poor learners. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 11171132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horohov J., & Oetting J. 2004. Effects of input manipulations on the word learning abilities of children with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 4365.Google Scholar
Kail R., & Leonard L. 1986. Word-finding abilities in language-impaired children [Monograph]. ASHA Monographs, No. 25.Google Scholar
Marton K., & Schwartz R. 2003. Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 11381153.Google Scholar
Montgomery J. 1995. Examination of phonological working memory in specifically language impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 355378.Google Scholar
Montgomery J. 2000. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 293308.Google Scholar
Munson B. 2001. Phonological pattern frequency and speech production in adults and children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44, 778792.Google Scholar
Munson B., Edwards J., & Beckman M. 2005. Relationships between nonword repetition accuracy and other measures of linguistic development in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 6178.Google Scholar
Munson B., Kurtz B., & Windsor J. 2005. The influence of vocabulary size, phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 10331047.Google Scholar
Tallal P., Stark R., & Mellits D. 1985. Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills. Brain and Language, 25, 314322.Google Scholar