Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T21:53:02.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Narrative production skills of language minority learners and their English-only classmates in early adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2015

PERLA B. GÁMEZ*
Affiliation:
Loyola University Chicago
NONIE K. LESAUX
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education
ANDREA ANUSHKO RIZZO
Affiliation:
Scholastic, Inc.
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Perla B. Gámez, Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, 1032 West Sheridan Road, Chicago, IL 60660. E-mail: pgamez@luc.edu

Abstract

This study investigated the narrative production skills of early-adolescent, Spanish-speaking language minority (LM) learners (n = 43) and their English-only (EO) peers (n = 38). The sample was born in the United States, educated in English, and representative of low- and high-income backgrounds. Using a picture book as a prompt, students’ narratives were transcribed, coded, and compared on macrostructure skills (story structure), microstructure skills (discrete language skills: vocabulary and grammar), and use of mazes (disruptions in speech). Results demonstrated that the groups did not differ on story structure. However, LM learners produced lengthier narratives than their EO peers, ones that resulted in stories that were less grammatically diverse and included more grammatical revisions and errors in prepositions. Thus, by early adolescence, EO and LM learners in urban schools may have well-developed macrostructure skills, yet the LM learners may still be developing specific microstructure skills.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adger, C. T., Wolfram, W., & Christian, D. (2007). Dialects in schools and communities (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Applebee, A. N. (1978). The child's concept of story: Ages two to seventeen. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.) (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Becker, A., & Carroll, M. (Eds.) (1997). The acquisition of spatial relations in a second language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., Fiestas, C. E., Pena, E. D., & Nagy, V. J. (2006). Cross-language comparisons of maze use in Spanish and English in functionally monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 233247.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science Public Interest, 10, 89129.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525531.Google Scholar
Capps, R., Fix, M., Murray, J., Ost, J., Passel, J. S., & Herwantoro, S. (2005). The new demography of America's schools: Immigration and the No Child Left Behind Act (Research Report). Washington, DC: Urban Institute.Google Scholar
Chomsky, C. (1969). The acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Coventry, K. R., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., & Valdés, B. (2012). On the first and second language acquisition of spatial language. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 12, 219230.Google Scholar
Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2002). Oral language expectations for African American preschoolers and kindergartners. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 11, 5970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cristofaro, T. N., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Lessons in mother-child and father-child personal narratives in Latino families. In McCabe, A., Bailey, A. L., & Melzi, G. (Eds.), Spanish-language narration and literacy: Culture, cognition and emotion (pp. 5491). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. G., Kline, S. B., & Snow, C. E. (1986). Definitions and definite noun phrases: Indicators of children's decontextualized language skills. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 1, 3748.Google Scholar
de Bot, K. (2004). The multilingual lexicon: Modeling selection and control. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1, 1732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children's bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16, 186202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (1991). Early literacy: Linkages between home, school and literacy achievement at age five. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 3046.Google Scholar
Duran, P., Malvern, D., Richards, B., & Chipere, N. (2004). Developmental trends in lexical diversity. Applied Linguistics, 25, 220242.Google Scholar
Evans, M. A. (1985). Self-initiated repairs: A reflection of communicative monitoring in young children. Developmental Psychology, 21, 365371.Google Scholar
Fiestas, C. E., & Peña, E. D. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: Language and task effects. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 155168.Google Scholar
Fought, C. (2003). Chicano English in context. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Fry, R. A. (2010). Hispanics, high school dropouts and the GED. Washington, DC: Pew Hispanic Center.Google Scholar
Fry, R., & Lopez, M. H. (2012). Hispanic student enrollments reach new highs in 2011. Washington, DC: Pew Hispanic Center.Google Scholar
Gándara, P., Losen, D., August, D., Uriarte, M., Gómez, M. C., & Hopkins, M. (2010). Forbidden language: A brief history of US language policy. In Gándara, P. & Hopkins, M. (Eds.), Forbidden language: English learners and restrictive language policies (pp. 2033). New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Garcia Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H., Crnic, K., Wasik, B., et al. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67, 18911914.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2002a). Command of the mass/count distinction in bilingual and monolingual children: An English morphosyntactic distinction. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 175206). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2002b). Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 207219). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2002c). Monolingual and bilingual acquisition: Learning different treatments of that-trace phenomena in English and Spanish. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 220254). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Genesee, F., Lindholm-Leary, K., Saunders, W., & Christian, D. (2006). Educating English language learners: A synthesis of empirical evidence. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2002). Narratives in two languages: Assessing performance of bilingual children. Linguistics and Education, 13, 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., Iglesias, A., Fabiano-Smith, L., Nockerts, A., & Andriacchi, K. D. (2008). Narrative transcription accuracy and reliability in two languages. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 178188.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 13681378.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49, 414.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Senor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, J., & Shapiro, L. (1991). From knowing to telling: The development of children's scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343365.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434446.Google Scholar
Jean, M., & Geva, E. (2009). The development of vocabulary in English as a second language children and its role in predicting word recognition ability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 153185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). Language development after five. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 307323). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J. (2008). Catching up or falling behind? Initial English proficiency concentrated poverty, and the reading growth of language minority learners in the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 851868.Google Scholar
Klee, T., & Fitzgerald, M. D. (1985). The relation between grammatical development and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 12, 251269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kormos, J. (1999). Monitoring and self-repair in L2. Language Learning, 49, 303342.Google Scholar
Kortmann, B., & Szmrecsanyi, B. (2004). Global synopsis: Morphological and syntactic variation in English. A handbook of varieties of English, 2, 11421202.Google Scholar
Kupersmitt, J. (2004). Form–function relations in Spanish narratives: A comparative study of bilingual and monolingual speakers. In Stromqvist, S. S. & Verhoeven, L. T. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Vol. 2. Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 395435). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, I. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts: Proceedings of the 1966 Annual Spring Meeting of the American Ethnological Society (pp. 1244). Seattle, WA: UWA Press.Google Scholar
Leadholm, B. J., & Miller, J. (1992). Language sample analysis: The Wisconsin guide. Madison, WI: Wisconsin Department of Public Health.Google Scholar
Lennon, P. (1990). Investigating fluency in EFL: A quantitative approach. Language Learning, 40, 387417.Google Scholar
Lesaux, N. K., & Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Exploring sources of reading comprehension difficulties among language minority learners and their classmates in early adolescence. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 596632.Google Scholar
Lesaux, N. K., Koda, K., Siegel, L. S., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Development of literacy of language minority learners. In August, D. L. & Shanahan, T. (Eds.), Developing literacy in a second language: Report of the National Literacy Panel (pp. 75122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.Google Scholar
Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Lopez, M. H., & Velasco, G. (2011). The toll of the great recession: Childhood poverty among Hispanics sets record, lead nation. Washington, DC: Pew Hispanic Center.Google Scholar
MacLachlan, B. G., & Chapman, R. S. (1988). Communication breakdowns in normal and language learning-disabled children's conversation and narration. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 27.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2010). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2010). Predictors of reading comprehension for struggling readers: The case of Spanish-speaking language minority learners. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 701711.Google Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2011). The gap between Spanish speakers’ word reading and word knowledge: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 82, 15441560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111151.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: A longitudinal study of parental styles in eliciting narratives and developing narrative skill. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 217253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McKee, G., Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2000). Measuring vocabulary diversity using dedicated software. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 15, 323337.Google Scholar
Melzi, G. (2000). Cultural variations in the construction of personal narratives: Central American and European American mothers’ elicitation styles. Discourse Processes, 30, 153177.Google Scholar
Melzi, G., Schick, A., & Bostwick, E. (2013). Latino children's narrative competencies over the preschool years. Actualidades en Psicología, 27, 114.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F., Iglesias, A., Heilmann, J., Fabiano, L., Nockerts, A., & Frances, D. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 3043.Google Scholar
National Center for Education Statistics. (2013). National assessment of educational progress 2013, reading assessments. Washington, DC: US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences.Google Scholar
National Clearinghouse for English Language Acquisition. (2011). What languages do English learners speak? NCELA Fact Sheet. Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Nippold, M. A. (2007). Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults. Austin, TX: Pro Ed.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Narrative competence among monolingual and bilingual school children in Miami. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 135174). Buffalo, NY: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 4158.Google Scholar
Petersen, D. B., Gilliam, S. L., & Gilliam, R. B. (2008). Emerging procedures in narrative assessment: The index of narrative complexity. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 115130.Google Scholar
Petersen, D. B., Gillam, S. L., Spencer, T., & Gillam, R. B. (2010). The effects of literate narrative intervention on children with neurologically based language impairments: An early stage study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 961981.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 18341849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Planty, M., Hussar, W., Snyder, T., Kena, G., KewalRamani, A., Kemp, J., et al. (2009). The Condition of Education 2009 (NCES 2009-081). Washington, DC: US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences.Google Scholar
Proctor, C., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 246256.Google Scholar
Rojas, R., & Iglesias, A. (2013). The language growth of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Child Development, 84, 630646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, C. (2013). Language use in the United States: 2011 (American Community Survey Report ACS-22). Washington, DC: US Census Bureau.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S., Rescorla, L., Tager-Flusberg, H., Fowler, A. E., & Sudhalter, V. (1991). The relation of utterance length to grammatical complexity in normal and language-disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 2346.Google Scholar
Squires, K. E., Lugo-Neris, M. J., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., Bohman, T. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2014). Story retelling by bilingual children with language impairments and typically developing controls. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49, 6074.Google Scholar
Stein, N. (1988). The development of children's storytelling skill. In Franklin, M. B. & Barten, S. (Eds.), Child language: A reader. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. D. (Ed.), Advances in discourse processes: Vol. 2. New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53120). Norwood, NJ: Albex.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1982). Children's concept of time: The development of a story schema. In Friedman, W. J. (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time (pp. 255282). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Stein, N., & Policastro, M. (1984). The concept of a story: A comparison between children's and teachers viewpoints. In Mandl, H., Stein, N., & Trabasso, T. (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 113155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Strömqvist, S., & Verhoeven, L. (2004). Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Rosston, K., Gerber, M., & Solari, E. (2008). Influence of oral language and phonological awareness on children's bilingual reading. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 413429.Google Scholar
Uccelli, P., & Paez, M. M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary development of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 225236.Google Scholar
Varon, S. (2007). Robot dreams. New York: First Second.Google Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Emergence of syntax: Commonalities and differences across children. Developmental Science, 11, 8497.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Strömqvist, S. (Eds.) (2001). Narrative development in a multilingual context (Vol. 23). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. (1984). Language production in foreign and native languages: Same or different? In Dechert, H., Mohle, D., & Raupach, M. (Eds.), Second language productions (pp. 1125). Tubingen: Gunter Narr.Google Scholar