Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T20:25:16.434Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sources of individual differences in the acquisition of tense inflection by English second language learners with and without specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 January 2014

ELMA BLOM*
Affiliation:
Utrecht University
JOHANNE PARADIS
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Elma Blom, Department of Special Education: Cognitive and Motor Disabilities, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands. E-mail: w.b.t.blom@uu.nl

Abstract

The goal of this study was to investigate whether individual difference factors influence the second language (L2) learning of children with specific language impairment (SLI) and children with typical development (TD) differently. The study focuses on tense inflection development in English L2 children. The roles of age of L2 acquisition, length of L2 exposure, and first language (L1) were examined. Twenty-four pairs of 4- and 5-year-old English L2 children with SLI and English L2 children with TD participated in the study. Children's responses on the third person singular and regular past tense probes of the Test of Early Grammatical Impairment (Rice & Wexler, 2001) were analyzed using logistic mixed regression modeling and classification procedures. For all children, those who started learning English later performed better than children who started learning English earlier, but the advantage of an older age of acquisition was particularly present in the L2 with SLI group. For children in the L2 group with TD, their accuracy with tense inflection clearly increased with longer L2 exposure, but this was not found for the L2 children with SLI. Finally, L2 children with TD were better able to transfer L1 knowledge than L2 children with SLI.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 675693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armon-Lotem, S. (2010). Instructive bilingualism: Can bilingual children with SLI rely on one language in learning a second one? Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 253260.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Adam, G., Blass, A., Fine, J., Harel, E., Saiegh-Haddad, E., et al. (2012). Verb inflections as indicators of bilingual SLI: Qualitative vs. quantitative measurements. In Leikin, M., Tobin, Y., & Schwartz, M. (Eds.), Current issues in bilingualism: Cognitive and socio-linguistic perspectives (pp. 179200). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1981). Second language acquisition from a functionalist perspective: Pragmatic, semantic and perceptual strategies. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Conference on Native and Foreign Language Acquisition (pp. 190214). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Bateson, M. C. (1967). Arabic language handbook. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. & Leonard, L. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 11851192.Google Scholar
Bhatia, T. K. (1993). Punjabi: A cognitive-descriptive grammar. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2006). Distinct genetic influences on grammar and phonological short-term memory deficits: Evidence from 6-year-old twins. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 5, 158169.Google Scholar
Bittner, D., Dressler, W. U., & Kilani-Schoch, M. (Eds.). (2003). Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition. A cross-linguistic perspective. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Blom, E., & Baayen, H. R. (2012). The impact of verb form, sentence position, home language, and second language proficiency on subject–verb agreement in child second language Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 777811.Google Scholar
Blom, E., & Paradis, J. (2013). Past tense production by English second language learners with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 281294.Google Scholar
Blom, E., Paradis, J., & Sorenson Duncan, T. (2012). Effects of input properties, vocabulary size and L1 on the development of third person singular -s in child L2 English. Language Learning, 62, 3.Google Scholar
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 421432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgemeister, B., Blum, L. H., & Lorge, I. (1972). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Butt, J., & Benjamin, D. (2004). A new reference grammar of modern Spanish (4th ed.). London: Hodder Education.Google Scholar
Chatterjee, C., & Hadi, A. S. (2006). Regression analysis by example. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V. (2008). The acquisition of the D-domain in child L2 Greek: Effects of age and construction. In Haznedar, B. & Slabakova, R. (Eds.), Child L2 acquisition: A generative perspective (pp. 97145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2011). Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, morphology and complex syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 223248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003) Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 10291037.Google Scholar
De Jong, J. (2010). Notes on the nature of bilingual specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 273277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, H., & Burt, M. (1974). Natural sequences in child second language acquisition. Language Learning, 24, 3753.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2006). Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 27, 164194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177190.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2007). Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: Constructivist account of morpho-syntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 224247.Google Scholar
Gaulin, C., & Campbell, T. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 5564.Google Scholar
Golberg, H., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2008). Lexical acquisition over time in minority L1 children learning English as L2. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Kreiter, J. (2003) Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 267288.Google Scholar
Haznedar, B. (2001). The acquisition of the IP system in child L2 acquisition. Language Acquisition, 23, 139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Phonological and visual short-term memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 11, 4556.Google Scholar
Hothorn, T., Hornik, K., & Zeileis, A. (2006). Unbiased recursive partitioning: A conditional inference framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 15, 651674.Google Scholar
Hulk, A., & Marinis, T. (2011). Internal and external factors in child second language acquisition [Spotlight issue]. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1 (3).Google Scholar
Hulk, A., & Unsworth, S. (2010). Bilingualism as a kind of therapy? Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 297303.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K., & Abrahamsson, N. (2003). Maturational constraints in SLA. In Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 539588). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Jacobson, P. F., & Livert, D. (2010). Past tense use as a clinical marker in older bilingual children with language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 24, 101121.Google Scholar
Jacobson, P. F., & Schwartz, R. G. (2005). English past tense use in bilingual children with language impairment. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 14, 313323.Google Scholar
Jia, G., & Fuse, A. (2007). Acquisition of English grammatical morphology by native Mandarin-speaking children and adolescents: Age-related differences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 12801299.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. (2007). Processing limitations and the grammatical profile of children with specific language impairment. In Kail, R. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 35, pp. 139171). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (2009). Cross-linguistic studies of child language disorders. In Schwartz, R. (Ed.), Handbook of child language disorders (pp. 308324). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Camarata, S., Brown, B., & Camarata, M. (2004). Tense and agreement in the speech of children with specific language impairment: Patterns of generalization through intervention. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 13631379.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Camarata, S., Pawlowska, M., Brown, B., & Camarata, M. (2006). Tense and agreement morphemes in the speech of children with specific language impairment during intervention: Phase II. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 749770.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C. A., Francis, D., Tomblin, B., & Kail, R. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 408428.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., McGregor, K., & Allen, G. (1992). Grammatical morphology and speech perception in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 10761085.Google Scholar
Lin, H. (2001). A grammar of Mandarin Chinese. Munich: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1987). Applying the competition model to bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 315327.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2008). A unified model. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 341371). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (1991). Cantonese: A comprehensive grammar. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2000). Grammaticality judgments in a second language: Influences of age of acquisition and native language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 395423.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2008a). Grammaticality judgments in children: The role of age, working memory, and phonological ability. Journal of Child Language, 35, 247268.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2008b). Differences in the cognitive demands of word order, plurals, and subject–verb agreement constructions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 980984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercer, S. A. B. (1961). Introductory Assyrian grammar. New York: Frederick Ungar Publishing.Google Scholar
Miller, C. A., Kail, R., Leonard, L. B., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416433.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2005). Effects of input rate and age on the real-time language processing of children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40, 171188.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2006). Real-time language processing in school-age children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 275291.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., & Windsor, J. (2007). Examining the language performance of children with and without SLI: Contributions of phonological working memory and speed of processing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 778797.Google Scholar
Oetting, J., & Horohov, J. (1997). Past tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 6274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. (2008). Tense as a clinical marker in English L2 acquisition with language delay/impairment. In Haznedar, B. & Gavruseva, E. (Eds.), Current trends in child second language acquisition: A generative perspective (pp. 337356). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010a). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Keynote article for special issue with peer commentaries. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 328.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2010b). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment: Response to commentaries. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 119136.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 213237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Sorenson Duncan, T. (2010). Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43, 474497.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. (2011). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Peets, K. F., & Bialystok, E. (2010). An integrated approach to the study of SLI and bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 9095.Google Scholar
Redmond, S. M., & Rice, M. L. (2001). Detection of irregular verb violations by children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 655669.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (2010). Evaluating maturational parallels in second language children and children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 320327.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38, 850863.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 14121431.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., Marquis, J., & Hershberger, S. (2000). Acquisition of irregular past tense by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 11261145.Google Scholar
Ringbom, H., & Jarvis, S. (2009). The importance of cross-linguistic similarity in foreign language learning. In Long, M. H. & Doughty, C. J. (Eds.), Handbook of language teaching (pp. 106118). West Sussex: Wiley–Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rispens, J. (2004). Syntactic and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia. Doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Saeed, J. I. (1993). Somali reference grammar. Kensington, MD: Dunwoody Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. L. (1999). Urdu: An essential grammar. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Strobl, C., Mallay, J., & Tutz, G. (2009). An introduction to recursive partitioning: Rationale, application and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging, and random forests. Psychological Methods, 14, 323348.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, S., & Baayen, R. H. (2012). Models, forests, and trees of York English: Was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and Change, 24, 135178.Google Scholar
Thompson, L. C. (1987). A Vietnamese reference grammar. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2005). Child L2, adult L2, child L1: Differences and similarities. A study on the acquisition of direct object scrambling in Dutch. Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Wode, H. (1983). One the systematicity of L1 transfer in L2 acquisition. In Wode, H. (Ed.), Papers on language acquisition, language learning and language teaching (pp. 144149). Heidelberg: J. Groos.Google Scholar
Zdorenko, T., & Paradis, J. (2008). The acquisition of articles in child L2 English. Second Language Research, 24, 227250.Google Scholar
Zdorenko, T., & Paradis, J. (2011). Articles in child L2 English: When L1 and L2 acquisition meet at the interface. First Language. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0142723710396797.Google Scholar
Zobl, H. (1980). The formal and developmental selectivity of L1 influence in L2 acquisition. Language Learning, 30, 4357.Google Scholar