Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T14:11:45.768Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive function and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Ugandan children with perinatal HIV exposure

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2015

M. D. Burkey*
Affiliation:
Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
S. M. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
P. Bangirana
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Makerere University College of Health Sciences, Kampala, Uganda
I. Familiar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA
R. O. Opoka
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Mulago Hospital/Makerere University College of Health Sciences, Kampala, Uganda
N. Nakasujja
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Makerere University College of Health Sciences, Kampala, Uganda
M. Boivin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA Department of Neurology and Ophthalmology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA
J.K. Bass
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: M. Burkey, M.D, M.P.H, Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, 550 N. Broadway, Room 907, Baltimore, MD 21231, USA (Email: mburkey1@jhmi.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is among the most commonly diagnosed mental disorders in childhood and is associated with substantial deficits in executive functioning and lost academic and occupational attainment. This study evaluates symptoms of ADHD and their association with neurocognitive deficits in a cohort of rural Ugandan children who were born to HIV-infected mothers.

Methods.

We assessed ADHD symptoms and executive function (including memory and attention) in a non-clinical sample of children born to HIV-infected mothers in rural eastern Uganda. Analyses included assessments of the psychometric properties, factor structure, and convergent and discriminant validity of the ADHD measure (ADHD-Rating Scale-IV); and executive function deficits in children meeting symptom criteria for ADHD.

Results.

232 children [54% female; mean age 7.8 years (s.d. 2.0)] were assessed for ADHD and executive function deficits. The ADHD measure showed good internal consistency (α = 0.85.) Confirmatory factor analysis showed an acceptable fit for the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5) two-factor model. Subjects meeting DSM-5 symptom criteria for ADHD had worse parent-rated executive function on six out of seven subscales.

Conclusions.

Our results demonstrate structural validity of the ADHD measure with this population, strong associations between ADHD symptom severity and poorer executive function, and higher levels of executive function problems in perinatally HIV-exposed Ugandan children with ADHD. These findings suggest that ADHD may be an important neurocognitive disorder associated with executive function problems among children in sub-Saharan African settings where perinatal HIV exposure is common.

Type
Original Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2015

Background

Over 60% of children in sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) are estimated to be at high risk for failing to reach their potential in cognitive development, with subsequent effects on educational attainment and poverty (Grantham-McGregor et al. Reference Grantham-Mcgregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007). The current study evaluates the importance of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) – a common neurodevelopmental disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) – among children living in SSA by assessing its association with executive function problems, an important and highly related domain of cognition (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013). A second goal of this paper is to address the lack of valid measurement tools for assessing child mental health problems in low-income country settings (Collins et al. Reference Collins, Patel, Joestl, March, Insel, Daar, Anderson, Dhansay, Phillips, Shurin, Walport, Ewart, Savill, Bordin, Costello, Durkin, Fairburn, Glass, Hall, Huang, Hyman, Jamison, Kaaya, Kapur, Kleinman, Ogunniyi, Otero-Ojeda, Poo, Ravindranath, Sahakian, Saxena, Singer and Stein2011).

ADHD is a syndrome of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and impaired attention (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Increasingly, ADHD is also recognized as a clinical problem related to underlying deficits in executive function (Willcutt et al. Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005; Mahone & Hoffman, Reference Mahone and Hoffman2007). Executive function is a developmental construct consisting of a combination of domains required for execution of purposeful actions; executive function has been defined as including attention control, cognitive flexibility, goal setting, and information processing (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002; Diamond, Reference Diamond2013). In their 2004 study, Biederman et al. identified higher rates of executive functioning problems in children with ADHD compared with non-ADHD controls (Biederman et al. Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero, Morgan and Faraone2004). Executive function deficits may be more prevalent among children with ADHD compared with other related child mental disorders. For example, compared with children with other externalizing disorders (e.g. oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder) but without ADHD, Clark et al. found that children with ADHD had increased impairment related to executive function deficits (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Prior and Kinsella2000).

Through its association with impaired executive functioning, ADHD affects the basic cognitive skills necessary for academic success and social functioning. Longitudinal studies show ADHD to be a robust predictor of poor academic achievement, grade retention, low occupational attainment, and psychiatric and conduct problems into adulthood, including substance abuse and incarceration (Wilens et al. Reference Wilens, Biederman, Mick, Faraone and Spencer1997; Leibson et al. Reference Leibson, Katusic, Barbaresi, Ransom and O'brien2001; Biederman et al. Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero, Morgan and Faraone2004; Biederman & Faraone, Reference Biederman and Faraone2006). Given these significant long-term health consequences, ADHD's association with increased medical costs, a high estimated global prevalence, and the existence of efficacious and cost-effective treatments, the World Health Organization (WHO) has identified ADHD as one of two worldwide ‘priority (mental health) conditions’ of early childhood (World Health Organization, 2003).

SSA is an important setting in which to study ADHD and neurocognitive development. In SSA, high rates of HIV infection and cerebral malaria combined with the effects of severe poverty, including malnutrition and stunting, to substantially increase the risk of neurocognitive developmental delays among children (Grantham-McGregor et al. Reference Grantham-Mcgregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007; Beddington et al. Reference Beddington, Cooper, Field, Goswami, Huppert, Jenkins, Jones, Kirkwood, Sahakian and Thomas2008; Zeegers et al. Reference Zeegers, Rabie, Swanevelder, Edson, Cotton and Van Toorn2010; Walker et al. Reference Walker, Wachs, Grantham-Mcgregor, Black, Nelson, Huffman, Baker-Henningham, Chang, Hamadani and Lozoff2011). High rates of poor cognitive and educational performance associated with these risk factors are proposed to be important factors in subsequent lost occupational attainment, high fertility, and intergenerational perpetuation of poverty (Grantham-McGregor et al. Reference Grantham-Mcgregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007). Executive function in particular appears susceptible to the kinds of environmental risks commonly found in SSA. A study in Mozambique showed that, among child development domains, language and executive function were most susceptible to the effects of extreme poverty (Fernald et al. Reference Fernald, Weber, Galasso and Ratsifandrihamanana2011). Prior studies in Uganda have revealed specific deficits in the executive function domains of working memory and attention among children with cerebral malaria (Boivin et al. Reference Boivin, Bangirana, Byarugaba, Opoka, Idro, Jurek and John2007).

SSA is also an important place to study child neurocognitive disorders due to the high prevalence of HIV and poverty (Grantham-McGregor et al. Reference Grantham-Mcgregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007). Theoretical and empirically observed risks to neurocognitive development are related to direct toxic effects of HIV (in infected children) and antiretroviral medications (Blanche et al. Reference Blanche, Tardieu, Rustin, Slama, Barret, Firtion, Ciraru-Vigneron, Lacroix, Rouzioux and Mandelbrot1999) on the developing brain, as well as the multiple social–environmental risks that commonly accompany maternal HIV infection (e.g. poverty, orphanhood, stressful life events) (Mellins & Malee, Reference Mellins and Malee2013; Llorente et al. Reference Llorente, Brouwers, Leighty, Malee, Smith, Harris, Serchuck, Blasini and Chase2014). Children with HIV infection and perinatal HIV exposure but no infection have been shown to exhibit high rates of emotional and behavioral problems, including hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention (Mellins et al. Reference Mellins, Smith, O'driscoll, Magder, Brouwers, Chase, Blasini, Hittleman, Llorente and Matzen2003; Nozyce et al. Reference Nozyce, Lee, Wiznia, Nachman, Mofenson, Smith, Yogev, Mcintosh, Stanley and Pelton2006; Mellins & Malee, Reference Mellins and Malee2013). The largest study of ADHD in HIV-infected children in SSA showed a very high prevalence (>24%) of all subtypes of ADHD, with duration of antiretroviral therapy a risk factor for greater severity of ADHD symptoms (Zeegers et al. Reference Zeegers, Rabie, Swanevelder, Edson, Cotton and Van Toorn2010). One of the few studies evaluating ADHD in children with perinatal HIV exposure found high rates of ADHD in children (drawn from medical clinics in New York City) with (21.8%) and without (11.6%) HIV infection (Mellins et al. Reference Mellins, Elkington, Leu, Santamaria, Dolezal, Wiznia, Bamji, Mckay and Abrams2012).

While there are few studies of executive function in children with perinatal HIV exposure, a moderately sized observational study in Thailand and Cambodia demonstrated intellectual and memory deficits in HIV-exposed but uninfected children compared with unexposed children (Kerr et al. Reference Kerr, Puthanakit, Vibol, Aurpibul, Vonthanak, Kosalaraksa, Kanjanavanit, Hansudewechakul, Wongsawat and Luesomboon2014). Another study found greater executive function deficits in HIV-infected children compared with uninfected children; the differences were explained by moderating treatment and psychosocial variables (Llorente et al. Reference Llorente, Brouwers, Leighty, Malee, Smith, Harris, Serchuck, Blasini and Chase2014). Taken together, children with perinatal HIV exposure represent a high-risk group for neurodevelopmental delays, especially when living in situations characterized by poverty, HIV-related stigma, and limited educational opportunities.

Despite WHO's recognition of the importance of ADHD globally and the presence of prevalent overlapping risk factors for neurocognitive delay in SSA, ADHD is often considered a disorder limited to high-income countries. A large, multi-national systematic review estimated the world-wide prevalence of ADHD in community settings at 5.3% (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde2007). Although the review included 102 studies, only four were conducted in Africa (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde2007). In a 2012 systematic review of ADHD studies in African settings, Bakare (Reference Bakare2012) identified only nine qualifying epidemiologic studies. In Bakare's survey, community- and school-based samples demonstrated ADHD prevalence between 1.5% and 8.7%, whereas clinical populations of children with histories of HIV, tuberculosis, meningitis, and intellectual disability found rates ranging from 45% to 100% (Bakare, Reference Bakare2012). In Uganda, the site of the current study, we are only aware of one study related to ADHD has previously been published. In this paper, Idro et al. (Reference Idro, Kakooza-Mwesige, Balyejjussa, Mirembe, Mugasha, Tugumisirize and Byarugaba2010) described transient ADHD-like symptoms following cerebral malaria infection. Despite epidemiologic evidence suggesting similar rates of ADHD across geographic settings, questions remain about the transferability of the ADHD construct to non-American or non-Western settings (Faraone et al. Reference Faraone, Sergeant, Gillberg and Biederman2003).

To address questions about the relevance of ADHD to children in African settings this study evaluated ADHD symptom patterns and executive function problems associated with ADHD symptoms in a group of school-age children living in rural Uganda. The sample for this study was anticipated to be at high risk for neurocognitive impairment (including executive function problems and ADHD) due to previously reported risk factors of being born to HIV-infected mothers and living in a region with high rates of poverty where malaria is endemic. While we anticipated high rates of ADHD and executive function problems in this sample, an accurate estimate of prevalence of these problems was beyond the scope of the current study. Instead, the primary aim of this study was to address the broader question of whether ADHD symptoms were associated with important and expected impairments in cognitive function (i.e. executive function problems) among Ugandan children with in utero HIV exposure. The secondary aim was to assess the construct validity and psychometric properties of a common ADHD measurement in our sample. The secondary aim addresses the programmatic concern of a paucity of validated instruments for child mental health in SSA and the conceptual question of to what extent a previously validated (in other settings) ADHD instrument measures a similar and coherent construct (Thompson & Daniel, Reference Thompson and Daniel1996) in a distinct socio-cultural context.

While behavioral rating scales are commonly used in the assessment of ADHD, there is little information available about the cross-cultural validity of such tools in SSA settings. To assess ADHD symptoms, we selected the ADHD-Rating Scale IV (DuPaul et al. Reference Dupaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid1998), a DSM-based parent-report scale that has shown good cross-cultural reliability and construct validity, albeit with little data from SSA. We hypothesized that ADHD symptom scores would be internally consistent and demonstrate a two-factor structure, similar to Western reference populations.

Based on previous studies of ADHD and executive function (Mahone et al. Reference Mahone, Cirino, Cutting, Cerrone, Hagelthorn, Hiemenz, Singer and Denckla2002), we hypothesized that ADHD symptom scores would be strongly associated with parent-reported executive function problems (similar construct, similar method), but would associate less strongly with performance-based assessments of inattention and impulsivity (similar construct, different method), and would not be associated with performance-based measures of general cognitive function (different construct, different method).

Methods

We collected parent-report and performance-based measures of ADHD symptoms and neurocognitive functioning in a sample of children in rural Uganda. This study was approved by the Michigan State University Biomedical IRB and Makerere University Research and Ethics Committee (Uganda). Research permission was issued by the Ugandan National Council for Science and Technology.

Study setting and subjects

This study was conducted in a community (i.e. non-clinical) setting in rural eastern Uganda. Uganda is a low-income country in East Africa that is home to 37.6 million people (The World Bank, 2014). Uganda has a limited mental health workforce with only 0.09 psychiatrists per 100 000 population, the vast majority of whom practice in urban areas (World Health Organization, 2011). In the study region, malaria is endemic, HIV is common, and rates of malnutrition are high.

Subjects for this study were recruited from an ongoing trial that includes caregivers and children ages 2–5 years old for a study of a caregiver training intervention on child neurocognitive development. Participants lived within a 20 km radius of Tororo town and were referred by the Infectious Disease Research Collaboration at Tororo District Hospital where the mothers had previously been evaluated and/or treated for HIV. As part of a supplemental study, up to four other children in each study home, ages 0–12, were recruited to be regularly assessed on similar measures to the parent study to identify potential dispersion of intervention effects to other children in the home. The subjects in the current study came from the supplemental study and include children between 3 and 12 years. All of the study children were born to HIV-infected mothers. The children's HIV status was not directly assessed as part of this study; however, the use of prophylaxis for opportunistic infections was available as a proxy and was 95% predictive of HIV infection status in a related study in the same community. Written informed consent was obtained from the child's primary caregiver and assent was obtained from children aged 7 and older.

Interviews were conducted by Ugandan researchers in the participants’ homes and/or the study office. All interviewers had completed at least bachelor's level academic training. Interviewers received two 1-week trainings by the study authors on caregiver assessments (focused on interviewing and scoring; trainer: JB) and child neurodevelopmental performance tests (focused on implementation and scoring; trainers: PB and MB) before data collection started.

Exclusion criteria for caregivers from the parent study included current suicidality or psychosis. Exclusion criteria for study children included: history of serious birth complications, severe malnutrition, bacterial meningitis, encephalitis, cerebral malaria, or other known brain injury or disorder requiring hospitalization.

Measurements

Baseline data were collected on demographics, clinical (health) characteristics, family characteristics, the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) (Caldwell & Bradley, Reference Caldwell and Bradley1984), ADHD symptoms using the ADHD-RS-IV (DuPaul et al. Reference Dupaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid1998), executive function as assessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) (Gioia et al. Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000), attention as assessed by continuous performance tasks using the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA) (Greenberg & Waldmant, Reference Greenberg and Waldmant1993), and general cognitive ability as assessed by the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children—2nd edition (KABC-II) (Kaufman, Reference Kaufman2004). All study assessments were translated into the local study languages: Japadhola, Ateso, and Luganda. Standardized translation procedures included: translation by a bi-lingual translator, piloting, and revision with a focus group representative of the study population (repeated as necessary until comprehension was achieved), back-translation to English and checking by two study investigators. The authors of the BRIEF also reviewed and approved all back-translations for the BRIEF. Modifications to the instruments mostly involved changes to idiomatic phrases, explanation of unfamiliar concepts, and replacement of terms for unfamiliar objects. (e.g., Japadhola does not have direct translations for ‘daily routine’ or ‘desk’ – benches are used in classrooms.)

The primary caregivers (91% mothers) were the informants for the ADHD-RS-IV and BRIEF and the children completed the KABC-II and TOVA. Parent report measures (ADHD-RS-IV and BRIEF) were collected by reading the questionnaire items to the caregivers, word by word. In case of lack of understanding, the interviewer was instructed to repeat the item to the caregiver as many times as necessary. In very few cases, caregivers did not understand in spite of repeated attempts and clarification was used to put the item in context. The ADHD-RS-IV and BRIEF (preschool or standard versions) were administered for all children, whereas the TOVA and KABC-II were administered only to children aged >5. All measurements were made prior to initiation of the caregiver training intervention.

ADHD-Rating Scale IV

ADHD symptoms were assessed using parent report on the ADHD-RS-IV, an 18-item scale that assesses the presence and severity of DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD, including a nine-item subscale for inattention and a nine-item subscale for hyperactivity/impulsivity (DuPaul et al. Reference Dupaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid1998). Scores on the ADHD-RS-IV range from 0 to 36 with higher scores corresponding to a larger number or greater intensity of symptoms. The ADHD-RS-IV has good test-retest reliability, established validity in cross-cultural settings, and good discriminative validity (e.g. compared with measures of disruptive behavior problems) (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Faries, Vowles and Michelson2005; Döpfner et al. Reference Döpfner, Steinhausen, Coghill, Dalsgaard, Poole, Ralston and Rothenberger2006). The ADHD-RS-IV has also demonstrated good reliability and concurrent validity with the Conners Teacher Rating Scale in pre-school age children (McGoey et al. Reference Mcgoey, Dupaul, Haley and Shelton2007).

Behavior rating inventory of executive function

To assess executive functioning, we employed the BRIEF school-age version (ages 5–12) (Gioia et al. Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) and BRIEF-Preschool version (BRIEF-P) (ages 3–5) (Gioia et al. Reference Gioia, Espy and Isquith2003). The BRIEF is an 86-item parent-report rating scale used to assess five theoretically and statistically derived domains of executive functioning. Children with ADHD have been found to score significantly higher on all five primary indices of the BRIEF-P (Mahone et al. Reference Mahone, Cirino, Cutting, Cerrone, Hagelthorn, Hiemenz, Singer and Denckla2002; Mahone & Hoffman, Reference Mahone and Hoffman2007). On the BRIEF, indices of greatest interest for concurrent validity [based on Mahone et al. (Reference Mahone, Cirino, Cutting, Cerrone, Hagelthorn, Hiemenz, Singer and Denckla2002) evaluation of the BRIEF in ADHD and non-ADHD children] included: inhibit scale, emotion control scale, and working memory scale. In order to combine pre-school and school-age versions of the BRIEF, we utilized t-scores to assess relative deviation from age-normed values. The back-translation of the final version of the BRIEF was approved by one of the test authors (Peter Isquith).

Test of Variables of Attention

TOVA (Greenberg & Waldmant, Reference Greenberg and Waldmant1993) is a computerized go/no-go test that utilizes visual and auditory stimuli to measure errors of commission and omission and response time. These metrics are combined into a composite ‘TOVA ADHD Score’. On the TOVA, children with ADHD have been found to have slow reaction times with high variability (Harris et al. Reference Harris, Schuerholz, Singer, Reader, Brown, Cox, Mohr, Chase and Denckla1995). Other evaluations using the TOVA have found good sensitivity, but low specificity for correctly identifying patients with ADHD (Lovejoy et al. Reference Lovejoy, Ball, Keats, Stutts, Spain, Janda and Janusz1999). The TOVA has previously been used in SSA to assess attention among children with a history of cerebral malaria (Boivin, Reference Boivin2002) and in Uganda among HIV-infected children (Boivin et al. Reference Boivin, Ruel, Boal, Bangirana, Cao, Eller, Charlebois, Havlir, Kamya and Achan2010) and healthy control children (Ruel et al. Reference Ruel, Boivin, Boal, Bangirana, Charlebois, Havlir, Rosenthal, Dorsey, Achan, Akello, Kamya and Wong2012). Ruel et al. (Reference Ruel, Boivin, Boal, Bangirana, Charlebois, Havlir, Rosenthal, Dorsey, Achan, Akello, Kamya and Wong2012) demonstrated worse reaction times and visual ADHD scores on the TOVA among children with HIV compared with HIV-uninfected children.

KABC-II

KABC-II is a paper-and-pencil assessment tool for general cognitive ability in children ages 3–18 (Kaufman, Reference Kaufman2004). The KABC-II's subtests minimize verbal instructions and responses in an effort reduce cultural biases in the assessment of cognitive ability. The Mental Processing Index on the KABC-II is a composite measure of general cognitive ability (with higher numbers indicating greater ability) and the KABC-II's Number Recall test assesses short-term memory (a component of executive function).

Statistical methods

Statistical analyses were undertaken to assess the internal consistency and factor structure of the ADHD-RS-IV, convergent and discriminant validity, prevalence of and risk factors for ADHD, and executive function deficits by ADHD status in the study population. Internal consistency was assessed using Cronbach's alpha. For factor analysis, we first used principal components analysis to extract initial factors and identify the number of factors to retain. Outcomes of exploratory factor analysis depend heavily on the number of factors retained and there are no established criteria to determine this procedure, so a number of decision rules were applied examining several solutions before coming to a final conclusion. The decision rules we used included: assessing the eigenvalues (selecting factors with a value above 1); graphically representing the eigenvalues to visually analyze the relative importance of the factors, where a sharp drop in the plot signals that subsequent factors are ignorable (scree plot); and using parallel analysis to adjust for sample bias in the observed eigenvalues (Dinno, Reference Dinno2009). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (using the DSM-5 two-factor solution of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity) were then performed and compared with previous findings from international samples.

The primary assessment of convergent validity was association between ADHD-RS-IV total symptom score and the BRIEF ‘inhibit’ scale [based on theoretical formulations and empirical data supporting inhibition of inappropriate thoughts or behaviors as a primary deficit in ADHD (Mahone et al. Reference Mahone, Cirino, Cutting, Cerrone, Hagelthorn, Hiemenz, Singer and Denckla2002)]. Secondary measures of convergent validity were associations between ADHD-RS-IV total score and other measures of executive function, including: (1) other BRIEF global indices of executive function; (2) TOVA ADHD Score, TOVA total percent commission errors, TOVA total omission errors, and TOVA response time variability; and (3) KABC-II Number Recall. The primary measure of discriminant validity was association between ADHD-RS-IV total symptom score and KABC-II Mental Processing Index.

To assess the relationship between ADHD and executive function, we used mixed effects linear regression models with ADHD-RS-IV total score as the dependent variable and the BRIEF indices and scales and the TOVA scores as independent variables. We also used mixed effects linear regression models to assess for differences in cognitive function by ADHD symptom level, using the KABC-II Mental Processing Index as the independent variable. We assessed whether children meeting ADHD criteria had greater deficit-level executive dysfunction (t-score ≥65) using χ2 analyses. As multiple children from the same caregiver were eligible for the study, we assessed the degree of clustering effects by family (e.g. due to heritability and use of the same rater on parent-report forms) by examining intraclass correlation coefficients. Since intraclass correlation within families was found to be high, we included shared primary caregivers as a random effect.

Mixed effects logistic regression was used to assess risk factors for binary outcomes. ADHD prevalence was estimated using standard DSM-5 symptom criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) based on ADHD-RS-IV scoring conventions (DuPaul et al. Reference Dupaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid1998). Acceptable level of statistical significance was pre-specified as p = 0.05 (two-sided). Data analysis was performed using Stata 12.0 (Stata Corporation, 1985–2013).

Results

Subjects

Two hundred and thirty two children were assessed; the mean age of the study sample was 7.8 (s.d. 2.0) years and 54% of the participants were female. Table 1 presents the demographic and clinical characteristics of the study sample and summarizes their scores on the ADHD-RS-IV and ADHD by sub-type. The mean score on the ADHD-RS-IV was 12.7 (s.d. 8.6). Using DSM-5 symptom criteria cutoffs, the proportion of children in our sample meeting criteria for ADHD (including all subtypes) was 5.6% (n = 13). The proportion of children with each ADHD subtype was: predominantly inattentive, 2.6%; predominantly hyperactive/impulsive, 0.9%; and combined (i.e. meeting criteria for both inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive subtypes), 2.2%. Of note, these categorical estimates of ADHD should be interpreted with caution since clinical cutoffs for the ADHD-RS-IV have not been established in Uganda.

Table 1. Summary of baseline characteristics and ADHD measures (n = 232)

Psychometric properties and factor analysis of ADHD-RS-IV

Cronbach's alpha for the ADHD-RS-IV was 0.85 for the full scale, 0.75 for the inattention subscale, and 0.73 for the hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale. [DeVellis (Reference Devellis2011) suggests that scores above 0.70 are ‘respectable’ and above 0.80 are ‘very good’.] There was no significant improvement to Cronbach's alpha when any individual items were dropped. Principal components analysis identified four factors with eigenvalues >1 (Factor 1: 5.29, 2: 1.24, 3: 1.13, 4: 1.03, 5: 0.98), while visual inspection of the scree plot (Fig. 1) showed a sharp shoulder after the first principal component, suggesting a one-factor solution. Using a parallel analysis (Dinno, Reference Dinno2009) we similarly found that a one-factor solution provided the best fit (Fig. 1). Since using eigenvalues alone has been shown to over-extract factors (Costello & Osborne, Reference Costello and Jason2005) and parallel analysis has been shown to be among the most accurate methods for factor retention decisions (Dinno, Reference Dinno2009), we continued with a one-factor solution. The single factor explained 84% of the covariance. Items that loaded most strongly on the factor included ‘forgetful’, ‘interrupts’, and ‘on the go’ and the least strongly included ‘blurts out answers’, ’ (has) difficulty sustaining attention to tasks’, and ‘difficulty playing quietly’.

Fig. 1. Scree plot of eigenvalues after principal components analysis for the ADHD-RS-IV. This figure illustrates the unadjusted and adjusted eigenvalues (y-axis) of each principal component (x-axis) for the ADHD-RS-IV. The dotted line (‘Random’) indicates the expected eigenvalues derived from a random dataset in parallel analysis.

In confirmatory factor analysis, the standard two-factor solution (inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity) showed acceptable fit indices [RMSEA = 0.059 (acceptable fit); CFI = 0.882 (poor fit); SRMR = 0.052 (acceptable fit)], suggesting plausibility of a two-factor model of ADHD. Measures of comparative fit suggested that the two-factor model was superior to the one-factor model (Akaike Information Criterion: 9567 v. 9571; Bayesian Information Criterion: 9746 v. 9750; CFI: 0.882 v. 0.877, respectively.)

Associations between ADHD symptoms and executive function

Greater total ADHD symptom scores were strongly associated with higher levels of parent reported executive function problems on the BRIEF (p < 0.001 for all scales and indices, including Inhibit scale, Global Executive Composite, Working Memory scale, and Metacognitive Index) (Table 2). All associations remained significant at the p < 0.001 level after adjusting for child age and sex (which were both associated with executive function deficits). Higher total ADHD symptom scores were also associated with worse performance-based measures of executive function on the TOVA (p = 0.01–0.04), but not with the TOVA ADHD Index (p = 0.92) (Table 3). Total ADHD symptom scores were not significantly associated with KABC-II Number Recall (p = 0.14) (Table 3). Total ADHD symptom score was not significantly associated with general cognitive function as measured by the KABC-II MPI (p = 0.15)

Table 2. Mixed effects estimate a of association between BRIEF b and ADHD-RS-IV ratings (n = 231)

a Mixed effects models included clustering by primary caregiver

b Data from the BRIEF were combined as t-scores from the BRIEF-preschool version (ages 3–5) and standard (school-age) BRIEF (ages 5–12).

c Data are combined from the ‘Emerging Metacognitive Index’ on the BRIEF-Preschool version and the ‘Metacognitive Index’ on the school-age (standard) form of the BRIEF.

Table 3. Mixed effects estimate a of association between performance tests and ADHD-RS-IV scores (n = 212) b

a Mixed effects models were adjusted for clustering by primary caregiver.

b KABC-II and TOVA evaluations were performed only on the subgroup of participants age 5 and over.

ADHD risk factors

Multilevel mixed-effects logistic regression analysis did not reveal significant associations between ADHD and any of the individual or family-level risk factors assessed. Factors evaluated included: sex, age, HIV proxy variables (i.e. on HAART or on opportunistic infection prophylaxis), anthropometric measures (z-score for height, MUAC), having a non-(biological) parent caregiver, both parents unemployed, total HOME score, and harsh parenting (an index of five items from the HOME related to harsh/punitive parenting practices). There were no significant associations between either opportunistic infection prophylaxis and ADHD (p = 0.44) or HAART and ADHD (p = 0.68). Of note, we were unable to assess odds ratios for history of malaria or preterm birth, since all children meeting ADHD symptom criteria had been exposed to malaria and none of the ADHD cases were born preterm.

To further investigate ADHD risk factors in this novel study setting, we conducted two additional sensitivity analyses (all accounting for clustering by shared primary caregiver) using: (1) an alternate categorical definition with subjects scoring in the upper 10th percentile on the ADHD-RS-IV classified as ‘cases’ (i.e. total score > 22; n = 27), and (2) continuous ADHD symptom scores as the dependent regression variable. In the first analysis, there were no individual or family level risk factors. In the second analysis, age, MUAC, and height z-score were significantly associated with ADHD symptom scores, whereas sex and HIV proxy variables remained non-significant. In adjusted regression models (including age, MUAC, and height z-score), only age remained significantly associated with ADHD symptom scores.

Executive function problems in children with ADHD

Children meeting symptom criteria for ADHD had significantly higher mean problem scores in all domains of executive functioning as measured by the BRIEF (see Table 4). In addition, they were more likely to score in the deficit range (t-score ≥65) on all indices and scales of the BRIEF (p≤0.001 for all comparisons) (results not shown). There were no significant differences in performance tests of executive function between children who met symptom criteria for ADHD and those who did not. These findings remained consistent in a sensitivity analysis defining children in the upper 10th percentile of ADHD-RS-IV scores as ‘cases’.

Table 4. Comparison of characteristics and executive function measures between ADHD v. non-ADHD subjects

a BRIEF scales and indices presented as t-scores.

b N for TOVA & KABC: ADHD (n = 12), non-ADHD (n = 200) as only children age 5 and over were assessed.

c Values indicated for these scales and indices are standard scores.

d Indicates standard regression models adjusted for clustering by primary caregiver; used when mixed-effects models failed to converge.

* p values for mixed effects logistic regression models included clustering by primary caregiver.

Discussion

Our study demonstrated strong associations between ADHD symptom severity on the ADHD-RS-IV and greater executive function problems in Ugandan HIV-affected children. Children meeting symptom criteria for ADHD were significantly more likely to exhibit deficit-level impairments in all measured domains of executive function on parent-reported measures. Overall, the findings of this study are comparable with studies in the USA and elsewhere demonstrating significant executive dysfunction in children with ADHD (Mahone et al. Reference Mahone, Cirino, Cutting, Cerrone, Hagelthorn, Hiemenz, Singer and Denckla2002). Therefore, our findings provide preliminary support to arguments that ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is likely to be relevant to SSA settings given its association with expected impairment and the coherent factor structure of parent-reported ADHD symptoms.

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the most comprehensive evaluation of executive function in ADHD in SSA to-date, and is the first to evaluate this subject in children with perinatal HIV exposure. Given the prevalent and wide-ranging neurodevelopmental difficulties of children with HIV infection and perinatal exposure that commonly include ADHD-type symptoms (Mellins et al. Reference Mellins, Smith, O'driscoll, Magder, Brouwers, Chase, Blasini, Hittleman, Llorente and Matzen2003; Wolters et al. Reference Wolters, Brouwers, Gendelman, Grant, Everall, Lipton and Swindells2005), developing valid assessment tools, accurate epidemiologic assessments, and a better understanding of disorder burden are urgent priorities for global mental health (Collins et al. Reference Collins, Patel, Joestl, March, Insel, Daar, Anderson, Dhansay, Phillips, Shurin, Walport, Ewart, Savill, Bordin, Costello, Durkin, Fairburn, Glass, Hall, Huang, Hyman, Jamison, Kaaya, Kapur, Kleinman, Ogunniyi, Otero-Ojeda, Poo, Ravindranath, Sahakian, Saxena, Singer and Stein2011).

The instrument used to assess ADHD in this study – the ADHD-RS-IV – demonstrated good construct validity and psychometric properties. There was good internal consistency and a factor structure comparable with international reference groups, DSM-5 subtypes (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and previous multinational studies using the same instrument (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Faries, Vowles and Michelson2005). The ADHD-RS-IV also showed good convergent validity with standard neuropsychological measures of executive function, including visual performance tasks. Our findings of greater executive function problems on the BRIEF and slower and more variable responses on the TOVA in children with symptoms of ADHD are consistent with previous findings in clinical samples (Harris et al. Reference Harris, Schuerholz, Singer, Reader, Brown, Cox, Mohr, Chase and Denckla1995; Mahone et al. Reference Mahone, Cirino, Cutting, Cerrone, Hagelthorn, Hiemenz, Singer and Denckla2002). The finding of no association between ADHD symptom scores and measures of general cognitive ability on the KABC-II supports the discriminant validity of the ADHD-RS-IV.

While our study was not designed to provide population prevalence estimates, a similar proportion of subjects in our study met criteria for ADHD compared with the community prevalence rate estimated in a large international meta-analysis (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde2007). Our estimate is lower than those described in previous studies in Africa (including the ones included in the above-noted meta-analysis), which have reported widely varying prevalence estimates (Kashala et al. Reference Kashala, Tylleskar, Elgen, Kayembe and Sommerfelt2005; Chinawa et al. Reference Chinawa, Odetunde, Obu, Chinawa, Bakare and Ujunwa2014) with a mean near 8% (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde2007). We are not aware of any previous evaluations of ADHD in perinatally HIV-exposed but uninfected children in SSA.

Our finding that the proportion of children in our study who met ADHD symptom criteria was similar to international prevalence rates does not appear to support the idea that HIV-exposed children in SSA represent a high-risk population for neurodevelopmental disorders. However, there are two important factors that limit our ability to accurately estimate ADHD prevalence in this study. First, our sample was not randomly selected and therefore prevalence estimates cannot be accurately estimated for the underlying population. Second, clinical cutoffs for the ADHD-RS-IV have not been established in this setting; therefore, categorical diagnoses are likely to be systematically over- or underestimated. In contrast, the poorer scores on the BRIEF and KABC relative to norms in the USA (see Table 4) suggest that executive function and general cognitive function were relative areas of weakness in our high-risk sample. However, the possibility of measurement differences on these instruments also emphasizes the need for local normative data in order to more definitively understand the burden of neurocogntive delays in this setting. Therefore, future studies could build on our work and establish the prevalence of ADHD and executive function problems and the role of HIV exposure in similar populations by: (1) establishing clinical cutoffs for the ADHD-RS-IV (which this study shows has good construct validity and psychometric properties); (2) incorporating population-based sampling (including a comparison group of children not exposed to HIV); and (3) establishing normative data for neuropsychological instruments.

In contrast to clinical samples that report two to three times as many boys than girls have ADHD (Willoughby, Reference Willoughby2003), our study found a slightly higher proportion of girls than boys met criteria for ADHD, though this difference was not statistically significant. This finding could partly be accounted for the differences between referred (clinical) and non-referred (community) samples, where gender differences are occasionally found to be negligible (McGee & Feehan, Reference Mcgee and Feehan1991).

A limitation of this study was that our diagnosis of ADHD relied on parent-report instrument since a trained child psychiatrist was not available to conduct clinical interviews. However, the instrument used has previously demonstrated excellent psychometric and construct and criterion validity across settings, including in studies of executive function (Mahone & Hoffman, Reference Mahone and Hoffman2007) and in a multi-national study including patients from South Africa (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Faries, Vowles and Michelson2005). Furthermore, in this study population, the ADHD-RS-IV demonstrated very good performance on key measures of reliability and construct validity, including convergent and discriminant validity.

A second limitation was that the statistical power to assess differences between ADHD and non-ADHD patients was limited by the small number of subjects meeting criteria for ADHD. A related limitation was the limited variability in our malaria exposure variable and HIV infection proxy variables (i.e. >97% reported a history of malaria and fewer than 10% were on HAART or prophylaxis for opportunistic infections) that precluded an accurate calculation of malaria or HIV status as risk factors for ADHD. Likewise, we were unable to assess risk due to perinatal HIV exposure since all included children were exposed.

Several strengths of our study should also be noted and include the availability of in-depth neuropsychological test results performed by trained researchers and the use of a community-based (as opposed to clinical) sample. This study also incorporated a rigorous evaluation of the construct validity of a widely used DSM-IV-TR-based scale [ADHD-Rating Scale-IV (DuPaul et al. Reference Dupaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid1998) – which remains relevant for the unchanged DSM-5 ADHD symptom criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013)] using validated questionnaire and performance-based neuropsychological tests administered by trained researchers. We also included comprehensive evaluations of possible medical risk factors [including HIV treatment status, history of malaria, and nutritional status (BMI)] and family level risk factors – including the validated and widely used HOME instrument (Caldwell & Bradley, Reference Caldwell and Bradley1984).

In conclusion, our study addresses the need for valid measurement tools for global child mental health; the ADHD-RS-IV is a useful and practical tool for assessing ADHD in Sub-Saharan African settings and could be used in future epidemiologic and treatment studies. Future research efforts are needed to clarify the normative equivalence of ADHD assessment tools in SSA settings to enhance their clinical utility in settings where psychiatrists are not available. Questions also remain about the contribution of endemic risk factors for ADHD in regions like Uganda where there are high rates of malnutrition, infectious disease, and extreme poverty as well as their impact on clinical presentations of ADHD.

Our study also demonstrates higher levels of executive function deficits in Ugandan HIV-affected children who met symptom criteria for ADHD compared with those who did not meet symptom criteria for ADHD. While we were unable to assess ADHD prevalence, our finding of increased executive function problems provides supportive evidence that ADHD may be an important and impairing neurocognitive disorder among children in other similar populations in SSA. While evidence-based pharmacologic and psychosocial treatments exist for ADHD, few studies have yet targeted ADHD in low-income countries or evaluated cognitive outcomes of interventions in HIV-infected or HIV-exposed children. A recent systematic review of mental health treatments for children and adolescents in low-income countries identified only two trials targeting ADHD (both of which evaluated the efficacy of psychotropic medications) (Klasen & Crombag, Reference Klasen and Crombag2013). Studies specifically targeting HIV treatment factors (e.g. through earlier initiation of antiretroviral therapy) have failed to improve child neurodevelopmental outcomes (Puthanakit et al. Reference Puthanakit, Ananworanich, Vonthanak, Kosalaraksa, Hansudewechakul, Van Der Lugt, Kerr, Kanjanavanit, Ngampiyaskul and Wongsawat2013). In contrast, an ongoing caregiver intervention trial in SSA improved overall cognitive development of preschool-age children with HIV infection though, ADHD and executive function were not specifically assessed (Boivin et al. Reference Boivin, Bangirana, Nakasujja, Page, Shohet, Givon, Bass, Opoka and Klein2013). Thus, there remains an urgent need to further define the burden of ADHD and to evaluate the potential impact of treatments for ADHD and related cognitive disorders among children living in SSA.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the U.S. National Institute of Health [grant number: 1R01HD070723-01 (MPI: Boivin, Bass)]. Dr. Burkey was supported by the Johns Hopkins Clinical Scholars Fellowship (5KL2TR001077-02). The authors wish to acknowledge the families who participated in this study and the many research assistants who contributed to data collection for this project. Part of this manuscript was previously presented as a poster at the Translational Science 2014 Annual Meeting, April 9–11, 2014 in Washington, DC.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-5. Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Anderson, P (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology 8, 7182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakare, MO (2012). Attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms and disorder (ADHD) among African children: a review of epidemiology and co-morbidities. African Journal of Psychiatry (Johannesbg) 15, 358361.Google ScholarPubMed
Beddington, J, Cooper, CL, Field, J, Goswami, U, Huppert, FA, Jenkins, R, Jones, HS, Kirkwood, TB, Sahakian, BJ, Thomas, SM (2008). The mental wealth of nations. Nature 455, 10571060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J, Faraone, SV (2006). The effects of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on employment and household income. Medscape General Medicine 8, 12.Google ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J, Monuteaux, MC, Doyle, AE, Seidman, LJ, Wilens, TE, Ferrero, F, Morgan, CL, Faraone, SV (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72, 757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanche, S, Tardieu, M, Rustin, P, Slama, A, Barret, B, Firtion, G, Ciraru-Vigneron, N, Lacroix, C, Rouzioux, C, Mandelbrot, L (1999). Persistent mitochondrial dysfunction and perinatal exposure to antiretroviral nucleoside analogues. The Lancet 354, 10841089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boivin, MJ (2002). Effects of early cerebral malaria on cognitive ability in Senegalese children. Journal of Developmental& Behavioral Pediatrics 23, 353364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boivin, MJ, Bangirana, P, Byarugaba, J, Opoka, RO, Idro, R, Jurek, AM, John, CC (2007). Cognitive impairment after cerebral malaria in children: a prospective study. Pediatrics 119, e360e366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boivin, MJ, Bangirana, P, Nakasujja, N, Page, CF, Shohet, C, Givon, D, Bass, JK, Opoka, RO, Klein, PS (2013). A year-long caregiver training program improves cognition in preschool Ugandan children with human immunodeficiency virus. The Journal of Pediatrics 163, 14091416. e5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boivin, MJ, Ruel, TD, Boal, HE, Bangirana, P, Cao, H, Eller, LA, Charlebois, E, Havlir, DV, Kamya, MR, Achan, J (2010). HIV-subtype A is associated with poorer neuropsychological performance compared to subtype D in ART-naïve Ugandan children. AIDS (London, England) 24, 1163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, BM, Bradley, RH (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. University of Arkansas at Little Rock. Little Rock: Arkansas.Google Scholar
Chinawa, JM, Odetunde, OI, Obu, HA, Chinawa, AT, Bakare, MO, Ujunwa, FA (2014). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a neglected issue in the developing world. Behavioural Neurology 2014, 694764 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C, Prior, M, Kinsella, GJ (2000). Do executive function deficits differentiate between adolescents with ADHD and oppositional defiant/conduct disorder? A neuropsychological study using the Six Elements Test and Hayling Sentence Completion Test. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 28, 403414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, PY, Patel, V, Joestl, SS, March, D, Insel, TR, Daar, AS, Anderson, W, Dhansay, MA, Phillips, A, Shurin, S, Walport, M, Ewart, W, Savill, SJ, Bordin, IA, Costello, EJ, Durkin, M, Fairburn, C, Glass, RI, Hall, W, Huang, Y, Hyman, SE, Jamison, K, Kaaya, S, Kapur, S, Kleinman, A, Ogunniyi, A, Otero-Ojeda, A, Poo, MM, Ravindranath, V, Sahakian, BJ, Saxena, S, Singer, PA, Stein, DJ (2011). Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature 475, 2730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, Anna B & Jason, Osborne (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical Assessment Research & Evaluation 10(7). Available online: http://pareonline.net/getvn.asp?v=10&n=7 Google Scholar
Devellis, RF (2011). Scale Development: Theory and Applications. Sage Publications: Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Diamond, A (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinno, A (2009). Implementing Horn's parallel analysis for principal component analysis and factor analysis. Stata Journal 9, 291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Döpfner, M, Steinhausen, H-C, Coghill, D, Dalsgaard, S, Poole, L, Ralston, SJ, Rothenberger, A (2006). Cross-cultural reliability and validity of ADHD assessed by the ADHD Rating Scale in a pan-European study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 15, i46i55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupaul, GJ, Power, TJ, Anastopoulos, AD, Reid, R (1998). ADHD Rating Scale—IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. Guilford Press, New York.Google Scholar
Faraone, SV, Sergeant, J, Gillberg, C, Biederman, J (2003). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: is it an American condition?. World Psychiatry 2, 104.Google ScholarPubMed
Fernald, LC, Weber, A, Galasso, E, Ratsifandrihamanana, L (2011). Socioeconomic gradients and child development in a very low income population: evidence from Madagascar. Developmental Science 14, 832847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gioia, GA, Espy, KA, Isquith, PK (2003). Behavior rating inventory of executive function, preschool version (BRIEF-P). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources., Odessa, Florida.Google Scholar
Gioia, GA, Isquith, PK, Guy, SC, Kenworthy, L (2000). Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology 6, 235238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grantham-Mcgregor, S, Cheung, YB, Cueto, S, Glewwe, P, Richter, L, Strupp, B (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. The Lancet 369, 6070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, LM, Waldmant, ID (1993). Developmental normative data on the test of variables of attention (TOVA™). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 34, 10191030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, EL, Schuerholz, LJ, Singer, HS, Reader, MJ, Brown, JE, Cox, C, Mohr, J, Chase, GA, Denckla, MB (1995). Executive function in children with Tourette syndrome and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 1, 511516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Idro, R, Kakooza-Mwesige, A, Balyejjussa, S, Mirembe, G, Mugasha, C, Tugumisirize, J, Byarugaba, J (2010). Severe neurological sequelae and behaviour problems after cerebral malaria in Ugandan children. BMC Research Notes 3, 104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashala, E, Tylleskar, T, Elgen, I, Kayembe, KT, Sommerfelt, K (2005). Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder among school children in Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of Congo. African Health Sciences 5, 172181.Google ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, AS (2004). KABC-II: Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. AGS Publishing: Shoreview, Minnesota.Google Scholar
Kerr, SJ, Puthanakit, T, Vibol, U, Aurpibul, L, Vonthanak, S, Kosalaraksa, P, Kanjanavanit, S, Hansudewechakul, R, Wongsawat, J, Luesomboon, W (2014). Neurodevelopmental outcomes in HIV-exposed-uninfected children versus those not exposed to HIV. AIDS Care 26, 13271335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klasen, H, Crombag, AC (2013). What works where? A systematic review of child and adolescent mental health interventions for low and middle income countries. Social Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiology 48, 595611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leibson, CL, Katusic, SK, Barbaresi, WJ, Ransom, J, O'brien, PC (2001). Use and costs of medical care for children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA: the Journal of the American Medical Association 285, 6066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llorente, AM, Brouwers, P, Leighty, R, Malee, K, Smith, R, Harris, L, Serchuck, LK, Blasini, I, Chase, C (2014). An analysis of select emerging executive skills in perinatally HIV-1-infected children. Applied Neuropsychology: Child 3, 1025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovejoy, DW, Ball, J, Keats, M, Stutts, ML, Spain, EH, Janda, L, Janusz, J (1999). Neuropsychological performance of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): diagnostic classification estimates for measures of frontal lobe/executive functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 5, 222233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahone, EM, Cirino, PT, Cutting, LE, Cerrone, PM, Hagelthorn, KM, Hiemenz, JR, Singer, HS, Denckla, MB (2002). Validity of the behavior rating inventory of executive function in children with ADHD and/or Tourette syndrome. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 17, 643662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahone, EM, Hoffman, J (2007). Behavior ratings of executive function among preschoolers with ADHD. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 21, 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mcgee, R, Feehan, M (1991). Are girls with problems of attention underrecognized? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 13, 187198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mcgoey, KE, Dupaul, GJ, Haley, E, Shelton, TL (2007). Parent and teacher ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool: the ADHD Rating Scale-IV Preschool Version. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 29, 269276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellins, CA, Elkington, KS, Leu, C-S, Santamaria, EK, Dolezal, C, Wiznia, A, Bamji, M, Mckay, MM, Abrams, EJ (2012). Prevalence and change in psychiatric disorders among perinatally HIV-infected and HIV-exposed youth. AIDS Care 24, 953962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellins, CA, Malee, KM Journal of the International AIDS Society 2013 . 16:18593 Available online: http://www.jiasociety.org/index.php/jias/article/view/18593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellins, CA, Smith, R, O'driscoll, P, Magder, LS, Brouwers, P, Chase, C, Blasini, I, Hittleman, J, Llorente, A, Matzen, E (2003). High rates of behavioral problems in perinatally HIV-infected children are not linked to HIV disease. Pediatrics 111, 384393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nozyce, ML, Lee, SS, Wiznia, A, Nachman, S, Mofenson, LM, Smith, ME, Yogev, R, Mcintosh, K, Stanley, K, Pelton, S (2006). A behavioral and cognitive profile of clinically stable HIV-infected children. Pediatrics 117, 763770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polanczyk, G, De Lima, M, Horta, B, Biederman, J, Rohde, L (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 942948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puthanakit, T, Ananworanich, J, Vonthanak, S, Kosalaraksa, P, Hansudewechakul, R, Van Der Lugt, J, Kerr, SJ, Kanjanavanit, S, Ngampiyaskul, C, Wongsawat, J (2013). Cognitive function and neurodevelopmental outcomes in HIV-infected children older than 1 year of age randomized to early versus deferred antiretroviral therapy: the PREDICT neurodevelopmental study. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal 32, 501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruel, TD, Boivin, MJ, Boal, HE, Bangirana, P, Charlebois, E, Havlir, DV, Rosenthal, PJ, Dorsey, G, Achan, J, Akello, C, Kamya, MR, Wong, JK (2012). Neurocognitive and Motor Deficits in HIV-Infected Ugandan Children With High CD4 Cell Counts. Clin Infect Dis. first published online February 4, 2012 doi:10.1093/cid/cir1037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stata Corporation (1985–2013). Stata Statistical Software Release 12.0: Programming. Stata Corporation: College Station, Texas.Google Scholar
The World Bank (2014). World Bank Open Data: Uganda Country Profile [Online]. Available: http://data.worldbank.org/country/uganda [Accessed 24 September 2014].Google Scholar
Thompson, B, Daniel, LG (1996). Factor analytic evidence for the construct validity of scores: A historical overview and some guidelines. Educational and Psychological Measurement 56, 197208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, SP, Wachs, TD, Grantham-Mcgregor, S, Black, MM, Nelson, CA, Huffman, SL, Baker-Henningham, H, Chang, SM, Hamadani, JD, Lozoff, B (2011). Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. The Lancet 378, 13251338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilens, TE, Biederman, J, Mick, E, Faraone, SV, Spencer, T (1997). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with early onset substance use disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 185, 475482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, Nigg, JT, Faraone, SV, Pennington, BF (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry 57, 13361346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, MT (2003). Developmental course of ADHD symptomatology during the transition from childhood to adolescence: a review with recommendations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44, 88106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolters, P, Brouwers, P (2005). Evaluation of neurodevelopmental deficits in children with HIV-1 infection. The Neurology of AIDS: Gendelman, HE, Grant, I, Everall, IP, Lipton, SA, Swindells, S (eds), Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, pp. 667682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (2003). Caring for Children and Adolescents with Mental Disorders: Setting WHO Directions. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2011). Mental Health Atlas 2011. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Zeegers, I, Rabie, H, Swanevelder, S, Edson, C, Cotton, M, Van Toorn, R (2010). Attention deficit hyperactivity and oppositional defiance disorder in HIV-infected South African children. Journal of Tropical Pediatrics 56, 97102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, S, Faries, DE, Vowles, M, Michelson, D (2005). ADHD Rating Scale IV: psychometric properties from a multinational study as a clinician-administered instrument. International Journal of Methods Psychiatric Research 14, 186201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of baseline characteristics and ADHD measures (n = 232)

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Scree plot of eigenvalues after principal components analysis for the ADHD-RS-IV. This figure illustrates the unadjusted and adjusted eigenvalues (y-axis) of each principal component (x-axis) for the ADHD-RS-IV. The dotted line (‘Random’) indicates the expected eigenvalues derived from a random dataset in parallel analysis.

Figure 2

Table 2. Mixed effects estimatea of association between BRIEFb and ADHD-RS-IV ratings (n = 231)

Figure 3

Table 3. Mixed effects estimatea of association between performance tests and ADHD-RS-IV scores (n = 212)b

Figure 4

Table 4. Comparison of characteristics and executive function measures between ADHD v. non-ADHD subjects