Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T05:50:30.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

EMBEDDED WH-QUESTIONS IN L2 ENGLISH IN INDIA

Inversion as a main clause phenomenon

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2014

David Stringer*
Affiliation:
Indiana University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to David Stringer, Department of Second Language Studies, Memorial Hall 310, Indiana University, 1021 E. Third Street, Bloomington, IN 47405. E-mail: ds6@indiana.edu

Abstract

This corpus study brings a second language (L2) research perspective, insights from generative grammar, and new empirical evidence to bear on a long-accepted claim in the World Englishes literature—namely, that inversion with wh-movement in colloquial Indian English is obligatory in embedded clauses and impossible in main clauses. It is argued that this register of Indian English is a L2 variety, functioning as part of a multilingual code repertoire, but that syntactic universals apply to first and second languages alike. Despite recent attempts at formalization, this distribution should be unattested, as such a grammar would fall outside the constraints of Universal Grammar and would contradict proposed discourse-pragmatic principles of natural language. A Perl program was created to search the Indian subcorpus of the International Corpus of English (Greenbaum, 1996) for relevant distributional patterns. Results reveal that wh-inversion in Indian English operates in the same way as in other varieties: It is robustly attested in main clauses and appears only occasionally in embedded clauses where syntactic and pragmatic conditions allow; it is obligatory only with interrogative complementizer deletion. Thus, contrary to the standard account but commensurate with recent corpus studies, users of English in India exhibit knowledge of universal constraints in this domain.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2009) Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59, 249306.Google Scholar
Adjemian, C. (1976). On the nature of interlanguage systems. Language Learning, 26, 297320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aelbrecht, L., Haegeman, L., & Nye, R. (2012). Main clause phenomena: New horizons. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Agnihotri, R. K. (1999). Regional report on India: English in India. In Singh, R. (Ed.), The yearbook of South Asian languages and linguistics (pp. 161183). London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
Balasubramanian, C. (2009). Register variation in Indian English. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bhatt, R. M. (2000). Optimal expressions in Indian English. English Language and Linguistics, 4, 6995.Google Scholar
Bhatt, R. M. (2008). Indian English: Syntax. In Mesthrie, R. (Ed.), Varieties of English: Vol. 4. Africa, South and South-East Asia (pp. 546562). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. (Reprinted from A handbook of varieties of English: Vol. 2. Morphology and syntax, pp. 1016–1030, by B. Kortmann, E. W. Schneider, K. Burridge, R. Mesthrie, & C. Upton, Eds., 2004, Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter).Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1997, October). Features and patterns in foreign language learning. Plenary presented at the Second Language Research Forum, Michigan State University, East Lansing.Google Scholar
The British National Corpus (Version 3; BNC XML Edition) [Electronic corpus]. (2007). Retrieved fromhttp://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/Google Scholar
Borer, H. (1996). Access to Universal Grammar: The real issues. Commentary on Epstein, Flynn & Martohardjono. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 19, 718720.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (2002). Main clauses are innovative, subordinate clauses are conservative: Consequences for the nature of constructions. In Bybee, J. L. & Noonan, M. (Eds.), Complex sentences in grammar and discourse: Essays in honor of Sandra A. Thompson (pp. 117). Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Muysken, P. (1989). The UG paradox in L2 acquisition. Second Language Research, 5, 129.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 98101.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J. (1991). The poverty-of-the-stimulus argument and multi-competence. Second Language Research, 7, 103117.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J. (Ed.). (2002). Portraits of the L2 user. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Cook, V. J. (Ed.). (2003). Effects of the second language on the first. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D. (2006). English worldwide. In Hogg, R. & Denison, D. (Eds.), A history of the English language (pp. 420439). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davies, M. (2008). The Corpus of Contemporary American English: 450 million words, 1990–present [Electronic corpus]. Retrieved fromhttp://corpus.byu.edu/coca/Google Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L. (2009). Second language epistemology, take two. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 291321.Google Scholar
Eckman, F. (1981). On the naturalness of interlanguage rules. Language Learning, 31, 195216.Google Scholar
Eckman, F., Moravcsik, E., & Wirth, J. (1989). Implicational universals and interrogative structures in the interlanguage of ESL learners. Language Learning, 39, 173205.Google Scholar
Emonds, J. E. (1970). Root and structure-preserving transformations (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Emonds, J. E. (1976). A transformational approach to English syntax: Root, structure-preserving and local transformations. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Emonds, J. E. (2004). Unspecified categories as the key to root constructions. In Adger, D., De Cat, C., & Tsoulas, G. (Eds.), Peripheries: Syntactic edges and their effects (pp. 75120). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emonds, J. E. (2012). Augmented structure preservation and the Tensed S Constraint. In Haegeman, L., Nye, R., & Aelbrecht, L. (Eds.), Main clause phenomena: New horizons (pp. 2146). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Escutia, M. (2002, May). Spanish learners’ production of English embedded interrogative clauses. Paper presented at Circulo de Linguistica Aplicada a la Comunicación (CLAC), Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain.Google Scholar
Finegan, E. (1999). Language: Its structure and use. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Garside, R. (1987). The CLAWS word-tagging system. In Garside, R., Leech, G., & Sampson, G. (Eds.), The Computational Analysis of English: A Corpus-Based Approach (pp. 3041). London, UK: Longman.Google Scholar
Gass, S. M. (with Behney, J., & Plonsky, L.) (2013). Second language acquisition: An introductory course (4th ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H. (1991). Language acquisition: A linguistic introduction. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Green, G. M. (1996). Pragmatics and natural language understanding. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Green, L. J. (2002). African American English: A linguistic introduction. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Greenbaum, S. (Ed.). (1996). Comparing English worldwide: The International Corpus of English. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, J. (1997). Projections, heads, and optimality. Linguistic Inquiry, 28, 373422.Google Scholar
Gupta, R. S. (2001). English in post-colonial India: An appraisal. In Moore, B. (Ed.), Who’s centric now? The present state of post-colonial Englishes (pp. 148164). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haegeman, L. (2012). Adverbial clauses, main clause phenomena, and composition of the left periphery: The cartography of syntactic structures. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haegeman, L., & Gueron, J. (1999). English grammar: A generative perspective. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hamilton, R. (1998). Underdetermined binding of reflexives by adult Japanese-speaking learners of English. Second Language Research, 14, 292320.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Casillas, G. (2008). Explaining frequency of verb morphology in early L2 speech. Lingua, 118, 595612.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y. (1997). The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The “failed functional features hypothesis.” Second Language Research, 13, 187226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, A. (1995). Belfast English and standard English: Dialect variation and parameter setting. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hilbert, M. (2008). Interrogative inversion in non-standard varieties of English. In Siemund, P. & Kitana, N. (Eds.), Language contact and contact languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hilbert, M. (2011). Interrogative inversion as a learner phenomenon in English contact varieties: A case of Angloversals? In Mukherjee, J. & Hundt, M. (Eds.), Exploring second-language varieties of English and learner Englishes: Bridging a paradigm gap (pp. 125144). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hooper, J. B., & Thompson, S. A. (1973). On the applicability of root transformations. Linguistic Inquiry, 4, 465497.Google Scholar
Institute of International Education. (2012). International students by field of study, 2010–12. Open doors report on international educational exchange. Retrieved from Institute of International Education website: http://www.iie.org/Research-and-Publications/Open-Doors/Data/International-Students/Fields-of-Study/2010-12Google Scholar
Ionin, T., Ko, H., & Wexler, K. (2004). Article semantics in L2-acquisition: The role of specificity. Language Acquisition, 12, 369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.Google Scholar
Johnston, M. (1985). Syntactic and morphological progressions in learner English. Canberra, Australia: Department of Immigration and Ethnic Affairs.Google Scholar
Kachru, B. B. (1985). The sacred cows of English. English Today, 16, 38.Google Scholar
Kachru, B. B. (1990). The alchemy of English: The spread, functions and models of non-native English. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. (Reprinted from The alchemy of English: The spread, functions and models of non-native English, by B. B. Kachru, 1986, Oxford, UK: Pergammon Press).Google Scholar
Kachru, B. B. (2006). World Englishes and culture wars. In Kachru, B. B., Kachru, Y., & Nelson, C. L. (Eds.), The handbook of World Englishes (pp. 446472). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kachru, Y. (1994). Monolingual bias in SLA research. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 795800.Google Scholar
Kanno, K. (1996). The status of a non-parametrized principle in the L2 initial state. Language Acquisition, 5, 317332.Google Scholar
Labov, W., Cohen, P., Robbens, C., & Lewis, J. (1968). A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto-Rican speakers in New York City: Vol. 1. Phonological and grammatical analyses. New York, NY: Columbia University.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (2012). Explaining matrix/subordinate domain discrepancies. In Haegeman, L., Nye, R., & Aelbrecht, L. (Eds.), Main clause phenomena: New horizons (pp. 159176). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2003). Stabilization and fossilization in interlanguage development. In Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 487535). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mair, C. (2003). Kreolismen und verbales Identitätsmanagement im geschriebenen jamaikanischen Englisch [Creolisms and verbal identity management in written Jamaican English]. In Vogel, E., Napp, A., & Zwischen, W. (Eds.), Ausgrenzung und Hybridisierung (pp. 7996). Würzburg, Germany: Ergon.Google Scholar
Marcus, G., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition [Monograph]. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(4, Serial No. 228), v–165. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McCloskey, J. (2006). Questions and questioning in a local English. In Zanuttini, R., Campos, H., Herburger, E., & Portner, P. H. (Eds.), Crosslinguistic research in syntax and semantics: Negation, tense, and clausal architecture (pp. 87126). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2009). Second language acquisition in early childhood. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 28, 534.Google Scholar
Mesthrie, R., & Bhatt, R. M. (2008). World Englishes: The study of new linguistic varieties. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). The acquisition of Spanish: Morphosyntactic development in monolingual and bilingual L1 acquisition and in adult L2 acquisition. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Mukherjee, J., & Hundt, M. (Eds.). (2011). Exploring second-language varieties of English and learner Englishes: Bridging a paradigm gap. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nagle, T. (2010). Language maintenance, language shift, diglossia, bilingualism? The changing roles of English in post-independence India. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Linguistics, Indiana University, Bloomington.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., & Supalla, T. (1990). A critical period effect in the acquisition of a primary language. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2005). For what and for whom is our research? The ethical as transformative lens in instructed SLA. Modern Language Journal, 89, 427443.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2013). SLA for the 21st century: Disciplinary progress, transdisciplinary relevance, and the bi/multilingual turn. Language Learning, 63(Suppl. 1), 124.Google Scholar
Platt, J., Weber, H., & Ho, M. L. (1984). The New Englishes. London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pozzan, L., & Quirk, E. (2013). Second language acquisition of English questions: An elicited production study. Applied Psycholinguistics. Advance online publication. doi:10.1017/S0142716412000690Google Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16, 103133.Google Scholar
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Linguistics, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, and Computer Science Department, University of Colorado at Boulder.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1997). The fine structure of the left periphery. In Haegeman, L. (Ed.), Elements of grammar (pp. 281337). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (2006). On the form of chains: Criterial positions and ECP effects. In Cheng, L. & Corver, N. (Eds.), Wh-movement: Moving on (pp. 97133). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ross, J. R. (1973). The Penthouse Principle and the order of constituents. In Corum, C. W., Smith-Stark, T. C., & Weiser, A. (Eds.), You take the high node and I’ll take the low node (pp. 397422). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Schachter, J. (1989). Testing a proposed universal. In Gass, S. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 7388). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schneider, E. W. (2007). Postcolonial English: Varieties around the world. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access model. Second Language Research, 12, 4072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, M. (2004). WordSmith Tools version 4.0. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sedlatschek, A. (2009). Contemporary Indian English: Variation and change. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 10, 209230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M. (1988). On the role of linguistic theory in explanations of second language developmental grammars. In Flynn, S. & O’Neil, W. (Eds.), Linguistic theory in second language acquisition (pp. 173198). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Singh, R. (Ed.). (1998). The native speaker: Multilingual perspectives. London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2008). Meaning in the second language. Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Sridhar, K. K., & Sridhar, S. N. (1986). Bridging the paradigm gap: Second language acquisition theory and indigenized varieties of English. World Englishes, 5, 314.Google Scholar
Stromswold, K. (1990). Learnability and the acquisition of auxiliaries (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Szmrecsanyi, B., & Kortmann, B. (2009). Vernacular universals and angloversals in a typological perspective. In Filppula, M., Klemola, J., & Paulasto, H. (Eds.), Vernacular universals and language contacts: Evidence from varieties of English and beyond (pp. 3353). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Thomas, M. (1991). Do second language learners have “rogue” grammars of anaphora? In Eubank, L. (Ed.), Point counterpoint: Universal Grammar in the second language (pp. 375388). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Trudgill, P., & Hannah, J. (2008). International English: A guide to the varieties of standard English (5th ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The interpretability hypothesis: Evidence from wh-interrogatives in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 23, 215242.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2005). Child L2, adult L2, child L1: Differences and similarities. A study on the acquisition of direct object scrambling in Dutch. Utrecht, the Netherlands: LOT Dissertation Series.Google Scholar
Verma, S. K. (1980). Swadeshi English: Form and function. Indian Linguistics, 41, 7384.Google Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Whitworth, G. C. (2007). Indian English: An examination of the errors of the idiom made by Indians in writing English. In Bolton, K. & Kachru, B. B. (Eds.), Asian Englishes (pp. 1255). New York, NY: Routledge. (Reprinted from Indian English: An examination of the errors of the idiom made by Indians in writing English, by G. C. Whitworth, 1907, Letchworth, UK: Garden City Press).Google Scholar
Wilson, A., & Rayson, P. (1993). Automatic content analysis of spoken discourse. In Souter, C. & Atwell, E. (Eds.), Corpus based computational linguistics (pp. 215226). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Rodopi.Google Scholar