Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T22:21:12.082Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cumulative contextual risk, maternal responsivity, and social cognition at 18 months

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2014

Mark Wade
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Chris Moore
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
Janet Wilde Astington
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Kristen Frampton
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Jennifer M. Jenkins*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jennifer M. Jenkins, Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto, 252 Bloor Street West, Toronto, ON M5S 1V6, Canada; E-mail: jenny.jenkins@utoronto.ca.

Abstract

By 18 months children demonstrate a range of social–cognitive skills that can be considered important precursors to more advanced forms of social understanding such as theory of mind. Although individual differences in social cognition have been linked to neurocognitive maturation, sociocultural models of development suggest that environmental influences operate in the development of children's social–cognitive outcomes. In the current study of 501 children and their mothers, we tested and found support for a model in which distal environmental risk, assessed when children were newborns, was indirectly associated with children's social–cognitive competency at 18 months through mothers' responsivity at 18 months. Part of this effect also operated through children's concomitant language skills, suggesting both a language-mediated and a language-independent mechanism of social–cognitive development. These findings are discussed with respect to the Vygotskian themes of internalization and semiotic mediation.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acock, A. C. (2005). Working with missing values. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67, 10121028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amato, P. R. (2001). Children of divorce in the 1990s: An update of the Amato and Keith (1991) meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 355370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 201271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aschersleben, G., Hofer, T., & Jovanovic, B. (2008). The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Developmental Science, 11, 862868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Astington, J. W. (1993). The child's discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W. (2006). The developmental interdependence of theory of mind and language. In Levinson, S. C. & Enfield, N. J. (Eds.). Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction (pp. 179206). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 13111320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayoub, C., O'Connor, E., Rappolt-Schlictmann, G., Vallotton, C., Raikes, H., & Chazan-Cohen, R. (2009). Cognitive skill performance among young children living in poverty: Risk, change, and the promotive effects of Early Head Start. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 289305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, H. N., DeOliveira, C. A., Wolfe, V. V., Evans, E. M., & Hartwick, C. (2012). The impact of childhood maltreatment history on parenting: A comparison of maltreatment types and assessment methods. Child Abuse & Neglect, 36, 236246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banyard, V. L. (1997). The impact of childhood sexual abuse and family functioning on four dimensions of women's later parenting. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 10951107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. In Baron-Cohen, S. (Ed.), The maladapted mind: Classic readings in evolutionary psychology (pp. 207239). East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 326339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bischof-Köhler, D. (2012). Empathy and self-recognition in phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspective. Emotion Review, 4, 4048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52, 664678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., & Flor, D. L. (1998). Maternal resources, parenting practices, and child competence in rural, single-parent African American families. Child Development, 69, 803816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Lerner, R. M. (Vol. Ed.) & Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Series Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (pp. 793828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Brownell, C. A., Zerwas, S., & Ramani, G. B. (2007). “So big”: The development of body self-awareness in toddlers. Child Development, 78, 14261440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buitelaar, J. K., Van der Wees, M., Swaab-Barneveld, H., & Van der Gaag, R. J. (1999). Theory of mind and emotion-recognition functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 3958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Zeisel, S. A., & Rowley, S. J. (2008). Social risk and protective factors for African American children's academic achievement and adjustment during the transition to middle school. Developmental Psychology, 44, 286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burchinal, M. R., Vernon-Feagans, L., Cox, M., & Investigators, K. F. L. P. (2008). Cumulative social risk, parenting, and infant development in rural low-income communities. Parenting: Science and Practice, 8, 4169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabrera, N. J., Fagan, J., Wight, V., & Schadler, C. (2011). Influence of mother, father, and child risk on parenting and children's cognitive and social behaviors. Child Development, 8, 19852005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1997). Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S. (1991). The validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Bates, E., Reznick, J. S., & Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of child language at twenty months. Journal of Child Language, 16, 239249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., & Estes, A. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deater-Deckard, K., Pylas, M., & Petrill, S. (1997). The Parent–Child Interaction System (PARCHISY). London: Institute of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers' emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 30, 928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Rosnay, M., & Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: Do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Rosnay, M., & Murray, L. (2012). Maternal care as the central environmental variable. In Mayes, L. & Lewis, M. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of environment in human development (pp. 5882). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dong, M., Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Dube, S. R., Williamson, D. F., & Thompson, T. J. (2004). The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 771784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dumas, J. E., Nissley, J., Nordstrom, A., Smith, E. P., Prinz, R. J., & Levine, D. W. (2005). Home chaos: Sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 93104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 13521366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Understanding others, and individual differences in friendship interactions in young children. Social Development, 8, 201219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 430457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ereky-Stevens, K. (2008). Associations between mothers' sensitivity to their infants' internal states and children's later understanding of mind and emotion. Infant and Child Development, 17, 527543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernyhough, C. (2008). Getting Vygotskian about theory of mind: Mediation, dialogue, and the development of social understanding. Developmental Review, 28, 225262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flouri, E., & Kallis, C. (2007). Adverse life events and psychopathology and prosocial behavior in late adolescence: Testing the timing, specificity, accumulation, gradient, and moderation of contextual risk. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 16511659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., & Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12, 201218.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallup, G. G. (1998). Self-awareness and the evolution of social intelligence. Behavioural Processes, 42, 239247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind & Language, 16, 494541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. W., & Schafer, J. L. (1999). On the performance of multiple imputation for multivariate data with small sample size. Statistical Strategies for Small Sample Research, 50, 127.Google Scholar
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6, 346359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilmann, J., Weismer, S. E., Evans, J., & Hollar, C. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventory in identifying language abilities of late-talking and typically developing toddlers. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 14, 40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: From toddlerhood through to the transition to school. East Sussex: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (2001). Acting nasty in the face of failure? Longitudinal observations of “hard-to-manage” children playing a rigged competitive game with a friend. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 403416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Deater-Deckard, K., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). “Speak roughly to your little boy”? Sex differences in the relations between parenting and preschoolers' understanding of mind. Social Development, 8, 143160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2006). Behavioural problems in 2 year olds: Links with individual differences in theory of mind, executive function and harsh parenting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 488497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Jaffee, S. R., Happe, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2005). Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Development, 76, 356370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., & Lecce, S. (2010). Early social cognition. In Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development & the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on Early Child Development, Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development. Retrieved from http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/Hughes-LecceANGxp.pdfGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., White, A., Sharpen, J., & Dunn, J. (2000). Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: “Hard-to-manage” preschoolers' peer problems and possible cognitive influences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 169179.Google ScholarPubMed
Ispa, J. M., Fine, M. A., Halgunseth, L. C., Harper, S., Robinson, J. A., & Boyce, L. (2004). Maternal intrusiveness, maternal warmth, and mother–toddler relationship outcomes: Variations across low-income ethnic and acculturation groups. Child Development, 75, 16131631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32, 7078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (2013). Understanding emotions (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T. J., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Benson, P. J., & Bullmore, E. T. (2001). Recognizing one's own face. Cognition, 78, B1B15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., DeVet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K., & Putnam, S. P. (1994). Maternal reports of conscience development and temperament in young children. Child Development, 65, 852868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Forman, D. R., Aksan, N., & Dunbar, S. B. (2005). Pathways to conscience: Early mother–child mutually responsive orientation and children's moral emotion, conduct, and cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Guttentag, C. (2008). A responsive parenting intervention: The optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal behaviors and child outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laranjo, J., Bernier, A., Meins, E., & Carlson, S. M. (2010). Early manifestations of children's theory of mind: The roles of maternal mind-mindedness and infant security of attachment. Infancy, 15, 300323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lengua, L. J., Honorado, E., & Bush, N. R. (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 4055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O'Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 561592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucariello, J. M., Durand, T. M., & Yarnell, L. (2007). Social versus intrapersonal ToM: Social ToM is a cognitive strength for low-and middle-SES children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 285297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez, C. R. Jr, & Forgatch, M. S. (2002). Adjusting to change: Linking family structure transitions with parenting and boys' adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matias, C., Scott, S., & O'Connor, T. G. (2006). Coding of Attachment-Related Parenting (CARP). Unpublished manuscript, King's College, Institute of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
McCabe, K., Houser, D., Ryan, L., Smith, V., & Trouard, T. (2001). A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53, 185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E. (2004). Infants' minds, mothers' minds, and other minds: How individual differences in caregivers affect the co-construction of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russell, J., & Clark-Carter, D. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development, 7, 124.Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 17151726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., Gopnik, A., & Repacholi, B. M. (1999). Toddlers' understanding of intentions, desires and emotions: Explorations of the dark ages. In Zaelazo, P. D., Astington, J. W., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Meunier, J. C., Boyle, M. H., O'Connor, T. G., & Jenkins, J. M. (2013). Multilevel mediation: Contextual risks, maternal differential treatment and children's behaviour within families. Child Development, 84, 15941615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622646.Google ScholarPubMed
Moore, C. (2007). Understanding self and others in the second year. In Brownwell, C. A. & Kopp, C. B. (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 4365). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Messinger, D., & Neal, R. (2000). Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 283298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Press.Google Scholar
Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2010). Mplus (Version 6). Los Angeles, CA: Author.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways into the community of minds. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 2649). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, K. E., Fox, N., & Mundy, P. (2005). Joint attention, self-recognition, and neurocognitive function in toddlers. Infancy, 7, 3551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Pretend play, mirror self-recognition and imitation: A longitudinal investigation through the second year. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 342365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okun, A., Parker, J. G., & Levendosky, A. A. (1994). Distinct and interactive contributions of physical abuse, socioeconomic disadvantage, and negative life events to children's social, cognitive, and affective adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 7798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pederson, D. R., Moran, G., Sitko, C., & Campbell, K. (1990). Maternal sensitivity and the security of infant–mother attachment: A Q-sort study. Child Development, 61, 19741983. doi:10.2307/1130851CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 12281238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Arbelle, S., & Mozes, T. (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children with schizophrenia, children with autism, and normally developing children. Schizophrenia Research, 42, 145155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prime, H., Perlman, M., Tackett, J. L., & Jenkins, J. M. (2013). Cognitive sensitivity in sibling interactions: Development of the construct and comparison of two coding methodologies. Early Education and Development, 25, 240258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raykov, T., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2004). Using the delta method for approximate interval estimation of parameter functions in SEM. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 621637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (1999). Social–cognitive development in the first year: Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rohrer, L. M., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Toth, S. L., & Maughan, A. (2011). Effects of maternal negativity and of early and recent recurrent depressive disorder on children's false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 47, 170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Devitt, K., & Crowe, E. (2006). What mothers say and what they do: The relation between parenting, theory of mind, language and conflict/cooperation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 105124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sally, D., & Hill, E. (2006). The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27, 7397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenkel, L., Marlow-O'Connor, M., Moss, M., Sweeney, J., & Pavuluri, M. (2008). Theory of mind and social inference in children and adolescents with bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine, 38, 791800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in “theory of mind.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 463471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvern, L. (1994). Parenting and family stress as mediators of the long-term effects of child abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 18, 439453.Google Scholar
Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 545564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology, 13, 290312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. N. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 489510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Symons, D. K., & Clark, S. E. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother–child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Social Development, 9, 323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77, 465481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trentacosta, C. J., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Gardner, F., & Wilson, M. (2008). The relations among cumulative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 12111219.Google ScholarPubMed
Velichkovsky, B. M. (1995). Communicating attention: Gaze position transfer in cooperative problem solving. Pragmatics & Cognition, 3, 199223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Happe, F., & Falkai, P. (2001). Mind reading: Neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage, 14, 170181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., & McKie, S. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29, 9098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). The collected works of LS Vygotsky: Problems of the theory and history of psychology including the chapter on the crisis in psychology (Vol. 3). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Walter, H., Adenzato, M., Ciaramidaro, A., Enrici, I., Pia, L., & Bara, B. G. (2004). Understanding intentions in social interaction: The role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 18541863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y.-G., Wang, Y.-Q., Chen, S.-L., Zhu, C.-Y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161, 153161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77, 640663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M., Lopez-Duran, S., LaBounty, J., & Hamilton, B. (2008). Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 44, 618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whipple, S. S., Evans, G. W., Barry, R. L., & Maxwell, L. E. (2010). An ecological perspective on cumulative school and neighborhood risk factors related to achievement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 422427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. H. G., Waiter, G. D., Perra, O., Perrett, D. I., & Whiten, A. (2005). An fMRI study of joint attention experience. NeuroImage, 25, 133140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of empathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 107130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar