Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T23:49:43.068Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The archaeology of language origins–a review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2015

Iain Davidson*
Affiliation:
Department of Archaeology and Palaeoanthropology, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia

Extract

Language is a feature to distinguish the ‘human’ from the ‘animal’ that has seemed a more enduring diagnostic character than some. But how is the breath of words to be made visible in the stony traces of the Pleistocene?

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Antiquity Publications Ltd 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arensburg, B., Tiller, A.M., Vandermeersch, B., Dudley, H., Schepartz, L.A. & Rak., Y. 1989. A Middle Palaeolithic human hyoid bone, Nature 338: 758–60.Google Scholar
Barton, CM. 1990. Beyond style and function: a view from the Middle Paleolithic, American Anthropologist 92: 5772.Google Scholar
Bateman, R, Goddard, I., O’Grauy, R., Funk, V.A., Mooi, R., Kress, W.J. & Cannell., P. 1990. Speaking of forked tongues: the feasibility of reconciling human phylogeny and the history of language, Current Anthropology 31: 124.Google Scholar
Belfer-Cohen, A. 1989. The appearance of symbolic expression in the Upper Pleistocene of the Levant as compared to western Europe, Etudes et flecherches Archéologiques de i’Université de Liège 32: 259.Google Scholar
Birdsell, J.H. 1977. The recalibration of a paradigm for the first peopling of Greater Australia, in Allen, J., Golson, J. & Jones, R. (ed.), Sunda and Sahul: 113–67. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H.. 1990. Transmission aspects of tool use in wild chimpanzees. Paper pre-circulated for the Wenner-Gren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications, Cascais, Portugal, March 1990.Google Scholar
Bordes, F. 1969. Os percé mousterien et os gravé achéuleen du Pech de l’Azé II, Quaternaria 11: 16.Google Scholar
Bordes, F. 1972. Tale of two caves. New York (NY): Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Bradshaw, J.L. & Nettleton., N.C. 1982. Language Lateralization to the dominant hemisphere : Tool use, gesture and language in hominid evolution, current psychoological reviews 2: 171 92.Google Scholar
Bunn, H. 1981. archaeological evidence for meateating by plio-pleistocene hominids from Koobi Fora and Olduvai Gorge, Nature 291: 574–7.10.1038/291574a0Google Scholar
Butzer, K.W. 1980. Comment, Current Anthropology 21: 635.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Piazza, A., Menozzi, P. & Mountain., J. 1988. Reconstruction of human evolution: bringing together genetic, archaeological, and linguistic data, peoceedings of the national academy of sciences 85: 60026.Google Scholar
Chase, P.G. & Dibble., H. 1987. Middle paleolithic symbolism:a review of current evidence and interpretations, Jounal of Anthropological Archaeology 6: 263–96.Google Scholar
Clark, G.A. & Lindly., J.M. 1989a. The case of continuity: observations on the bioculturaltransition in Europe and Western Asia, in Mellars & Stringer (ed.): 626 76.Google Scholar
Clark, G.A. 1989b. Modern Human Origins In The Levant and Western Asia: the fossil and archeological evidence. American Anthropologist 91: 964–85.10.1525/aa.1989.91.4.02a00090Google Scholar
Davidson, I. 1989. Freedom Of informatioN, in Morphy, H. (ed.). Animals into art: 440–56. London: Unwin Hyman.Google Scholar
Davidson, I. 1990. Bilzingsleben and early marking. Rock Art Research 7: 526.Google Scholar
Davidson, I.In Press. Naming and dating in ihe Upper Palaeolithic Of Spain, in Clark, G.A. (ed.). Perspectives on the past: Paradigmatic biases in Mediterranean hunter-gatherer research: 205–15. Philadelphia (Pa): University of Pennsylvania press.Google Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble., W. 1989. The archaeology of perception: traces of depiction and language, Current Anthropology 30: 125–55.Google Scholar
Davidson, I. 1990. Tools, humans, and evolution: the relevance of the Upper Palaeolithic. Paper precirculated for the WennerGren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications, Cascais, Portugal, March 1990.Google Scholar
Davidson, I., Sutton, S.A. & Cook., N.d., n.Fossils exposed in stone artefacts: an Australian example. Submitted to Journal of Human Evolution.Google Scholar
Delluc, B. & Delluc., G. 1978. Les manifestations graphiques aurignaciennes sur support rocheux des environs des Eyzies (Dordogne), Gallia Pré-histoire 21: 213438.Google Scholar
Dibble, H.L. 1989. The implications of stone tools types for the presence of language during the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic, in Mellars & Stringer (ed.): 415–32.Google Scholar
Falk, D. 1987. Hominid paleoneurology, Annual Review of Anthropology 16: 1330.Google Scholar
Foley, R.A. 1987. Hominid species and stone tool assemblages: how are they related?, Antiquity 61: 380–92.Google Scholar
Foley, R.A. 1989. The ecological conditions of speciation: a comparative approach to the origin of anatomically-modern humans, in Mellars & Stringer (ed.): 298318.Google Scholar
Fridrich, J. 1976. Příspěvek k problematice počátků uměléckeho a estetického cítění u paleantropů, Pamatky Archeologicke 68: 527.Google Scholar
Gardner B, T & Gardner., R.A. 1969. Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee, Science 165: 664–72.Google Scholar
Gautier, A. 1986. Unehistoire de dents: les soidisant incisives travaillées du paléolithique moyen de Sclayn Helenium 26: 177–81.Google Scholar
Gibson, J.J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J. 1984a. Ascent to civilization. London: Collins.Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J. 1984b. Mental abilities of early man: a look at some hard evidence, in Foley, R.A. (ed.), Hominid evolution and community ecology: 167–90. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gowlett, J.A.J. 1986. Culture and conceptualization: the Oldowan-Acheulian gradient, in Bailey, G.N. & Callow, P. (ed.), Stone Age prehistory: 243–60. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Groves, C.P. 1989. A theory of human and primate evolution. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Hahn, J. 1986. Kraft und Agression: die Botschaft der Eiszeitkunst Im Aurignucien Suddeutschlands? Tübingen: Institut für Lirgeschichte der Universitat Tübingen.Google Scholar
Hayden, B. 1989. From chopper to celt: the evolution of resharpening, in Torrence, R. (ed.), time, energy and stone tools: 716. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, C.F. 1959. Animal ‘languages’ and human Language, in Spuhler, J.N. (ed.), The evolution Of Man’S capacity for culture: 329. Detroit (Mi): Wayne State University Press.Google Scholar
Holloway, R.L. 1969. Culture: A human domain, Current Anthropology 10: 395412.Google Scholar
Holloway, R.L. 1983. Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior, Human Neurobiology 2: 105–14.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. 1989. The social and environmental relations of human beings and other animals, In Standen, V. & Foley, R.A. (ed.). Comparative Socioecology: the behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals: 495512. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. 1990. Tool-Use, Sociality And Intelligence. Paper Pre-Circulated For The Wenner-Gren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications, cascais, portugal, March 1990.Google Scholar
Isaac, G.LL. 1976. Stages of cultural elaboration in the pleistocene: possible archeological indicators of the development of language, in Hamad, S.R., Steklis, H.D. & Lancaster, J. (ed.), Origins and evolution of language and speech, Annals of the New York Academy Of Sciences 280: 275–88.Google Scholar
Isaac, G.LL. 1984 The archaeology Of human origins: studies Of the lower pleistocene in east Africa 1971-1981, in Advances in World Archaeology 3: 187.Google Scholar
Jones, P.R. 1981. Experimental implement manufacture and use; a case study from olduvai Gorge, Tanzania, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Of London B 292: 189–95.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 1990. Human gesture (With Postscript). Paper Pre-Circulated for the Wenner-Gren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications, Cascais, Portugal, March 1990.Google Scholar
Laitman, J.T. 1983. The evolution of the hominid upper respiratory system and the implications for the Origins Of Speech, in De, E. Grolier (ed.), Glossogenetics: the Origin and evolution Of language: 6390. Paris: Harwood Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 1984. The biology and evolution Of language. Cambridge (Ma): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 1989. The origins of some aspects of human language and cognition, in Mellars & Stringer (ed.): 391414.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Laitman, J.T., Reidenberg, J.S., Land-Ahl, K. & Gannon., P.J. 1989. Folk Physiology and talking hyoids, Nature 342: 486–7.Google Scholar
Lindly, J.M. & Clark., C.A. 1990. Symbolism and modern human Origins, Current Anthropology 31: 233–61.Google Scholar
Mania, D. & Mania., U. 1988. Deliberate engravins On bone artefacts of homo erectus, Rock Art Research 5(2): 91107.Google Scholar
Marler, P. 1976. Social Organization, Communication and graded signals: the chimpanzee and the gorilla, in Bateson, P.P.G. & Hinde, R.A. (Ed.), Growing Points in ethology: 195237. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marshack, A. 1976. some symbolic implications of the Paleolithic Symbolic evidence for the origin of language, Current Anthropology 17(2): 274–82.10.1086/201716Google Scholar
Marshack, A. 1977. The Meander As A System: the analysis and recognition of iconographic units in upper palaeolithic compositions, in Ucko, P.J. (ed.), Form in indigenous art: 286317. Canberra: Australian Institute Of Aboriginal Studies.Google Scholar
Marshack, A. 1989. Evolution Of the human capacity: the Symbolic evidence, Yearbook Of Physical Anthropology 32: 134.Google Scholar
Martin, H. 1908. Nouvelles constatations sur les os utiliséS à l’éPoque moustérienne, Bulletin de la société préhistorique française 1908: 108115.Google Scholar
Mcburney, C.B.M. 1969. The Haua Fteah (Cyrenaica) and the Stone Age of the south-east Mediterranean. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mellars, P. 1989. Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. Current Anthropology 30: 349–85.Google Scholar
Mellars, P. & Stringer, C. (ed.). 1989. The human revolution: behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Noble, W. 1981. Gibsonian theory and the pragmatist perspective. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 11: 6585.Google Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson., I. 1989. On depiction and language. Current Anthropology 30: 337–42.Google Scholar
Noble, W. 1990. Tracing the origins of modern human behaviour: methodological pitfalls, in Davis, W. (ed.), The very possibility of meaning. Albuquerque (NM): University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Oakley, K.P. 1973. fossil shell observed by Acheulian man. Antiquity 47: 5990.Google Scholar
Oakley, K.P. 1981. Emergence of higher thought 3.0-0.2 Ma BP. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 292: 205–11 Google Scholar
Parker, S.T. 1985. A social-technological model for the evolution of language, Current Anthropology 26: 617–39.Google Scholar
Parker, S.T. & Gibson., K.R. 1979. A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 367408.Google Scholar
Pellkgrin, I. 1990. A framework for analysing prehistoric stone tool manufacture and a tentative application to some early lithic industries. Paper delivered at the Versailles Colloquium of the Foundation Fyssen, 25-29 November 1988. MS held at CNRS, Meudon.Google Scholar
Pilbeam, D. 1986. The origin of Ffomo sapiens: the fossil evidence, in Wood, B., Martin, L. & Andrews, P. (ed.), Major topics in primate and human evolution: 331–8. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Potts, R. 1989. Olorgesailie: new excavations and findings in Early and Middle Pleistocene contexts, southern Kenya rift valley, Journal of Human Evolution 18: 477–84.Google Scholar
Potts, R. & Shipman., P. 1981. Cutmarks made by stone tools on bones from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania, Nature 291: 577–80.Google Scholar
Premack, D. 1986. Gavagai! or the future history of the animal language controversy. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, M.B. 1986. Excavation of the Lower Palaeolithic site at Amey’s Eartham Pit, Boxgrove, West Sussex: a preliminary report, Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 52: 215–45.Google Scholar
Roberts, R.G., R., Jones & Smith, M.A.. 1990. Thermo-luminescence dating of a 50,000-year-old human occupation site in northern Australia, Nature 345: 153–6.Google Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. (ed.). 1977. Language learning by a chimpanzee. New York (NY): Academic Press.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S.Rumbaugh., D.M.Smith, S.T. & Lawson, J.. 1980. Reference: the linguistic essential. Science 210: 922–5.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S.Romski., M.A.Hopkins, W.D. & Sevcik, R.A.. 1989. Symbol acquisition and use by Pan troglodytes. Pan paniscus. Homo sapiens, in Heltne, P.G. & Marquardt, L.A. (ed.). Understanding chimpanzees: 266–95. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schick, K. 1987. Modeling the formation of Early Stone Age artifact concentrations. Journal of Human Evolution 16: 789807.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R.M.Cheney, D.L. & Marler., P. 1980. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence for predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210: 801–3.Google Scholar
Smith, F.H.Falsetti, A.B. & Donnelly., S.M. 1989. Modern human origins. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 32: 3568.Google Scholar
Snowdon., C.T. 1990. Language parallels in animal communication: the emperor’s new clothes? Paper pre-circulated for the Wenner-Gren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications. Cascais. Portugal. March 1990.Google Scholar
Stringer, C.B. & Andrews., P. 1988a. Genetic and fossil evidence for the origin of modern humans. Science 239: 12638.Google Scholar
Stringer, C.B. 1988b. Response. Science 241: 773–4.Google Scholar
Taborin, Y. 1990. Les prentices de la parure. in Farizy, C. (ed.). Paléolithique Moyen Recent ei Paléolithique Supérieur Ancien en Europe: ruptures et transitions: examen critique des documents archéologiques: 335–44. Paris: CNRS.Google Scholar
Toth, N. & Schick., K. 1990. Early stone technologies and linguistic/cognitive inferences. Paper pre-circulated for the Wenner-Gren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications, Cascais, Portugal, March 1990.Google Scholar
Vycotsky, L.S. 1978 [1930]. Mastery of memory and thinking, in Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S. & Souberman, E. (ed.), Mind in society: 3851. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walls, G.L. 1942. The vertebrate eye and its adaptive radiation. Bloomfield Hills (MI): Cranbrook Press. Cranbrook Institute of Science Bulletin 19.Google Scholar
Wenban-Smith, F.F. 1989. The use of canonical variates for determination of biface manufacturing technology at Boxgrove Lower Palaeolithic site and the behavioural implications of this technology, Journal of Archaeological Science 16: 1726.Google Scholar
Wetzel, R & Bosinski, G. (ed.). 1969. Die Bockstein-schmiede im Lonetal (Markung Rammionge, Kr, Ulm). Stuttgart: Veröffentlichen der Staatliche Amtes fur Denkmalpflege.Google Scholar
Whallon, R. 1989. Elements of cultural change in the later Palaeolithic, in Mellars & Stringer (ed.): 433–54.Google Scholar
White, R. 1985. Thoughts on social relationships and language in hominid evolution, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 2: 95115.Google Scholar
White, R. 1989. Production complexity and standardization in early Aurignacian bead and pendant manufacture: evolutionary implications, in Mellars & Stringer (ed.): 366–90.Google Scholar
Willouchby, P. 1985. Spheroids and battered stones in the African early Stone Age, World Archaeology 17: 4460.Google Scholar
Wolpoff, M.H., Spuhler, J.N., Smith, F.H., Radovcic, J., Pope, G., Frayer, D.W., Eckhardt, R. & Clark., G. 1988. Modern human origins, Science 241: 772–3.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. 1990. Layers of thinking in tool behavior. Paper precirculated for the Wenner-Gren Symposium Tools, language and intelligence: evolutionary implications, Cascais, Portugal. March 1990.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. & Mcgrew., W.C. 1989. An ape’s eye view of the Oldowan, Man 24: 383–98.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. & Tierson., F. 1990. Regional comparison of the shapes of Later Acheulean handaxes, American Anthropologist 92: 7384.Google Scholar