Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T06:52:42.969Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Investigating eye movement patterns, language, and social ability in children with autism spectrum disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 March 2014

Steven D. Stagg*
Affiliation:
Anglia Ruskin University
Karina J. Linnell
Affiliation:
Goldsmiths, University of London
Pamela Heaton
Affiliation:
Goldsmiths, University of London
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Steven Stagg, Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin University, East Road, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK; E-mail: steven.stagg@anglia.ac.uk.

Abstract

Although all intellectually high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) display core social and communication deficits, some develop language within a normative timescale and others experience significant delays and subsequent language impairment. Early attention to social stimuli plays an important role in the emergence of language, and reduced attention to faces has been documented in infants later diagnosed with ASD. We investigated the extent to which patterns of attention to social stimuli would differentiate early and late language onset groups. Children with ASD (mean age = 10 years) differing on language onset timing (late/normal) and a typically developing comparison group completed a task in which visual attention to interacting and noninteracting human figures was mapped using eye tracking. Correlations on visual attention data and results from tests measuring current social and language ability were conducted. Patterns of visual attention did not distinguish typically developing children and ASD children with normal language onset. Children with ASD and late language onset showed significantly reduced attention to salient social stimuli. Associations between current language ability and social attention were observed. Delay in language onset is associated with current language skills as well as with specific eye-tracking patterns.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Belmonte, M. K., Gomot, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Visual attention in autism families: Unaffected sibs share atypical frontal activation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 259276. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02153.x Google Scholar
Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Social attention and real-world scenes: The roles of action, competition and social content. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 986998.Google Scholar
Bonnel, A., McAdams, S., Smith, B., Berthiaume, C., Bertone, A., Ciocca, V., et al. (2010). Enhanced pure-tone pitch discrimination among persons with autism but not Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48, 24652475. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.020 Google Scholar
Britton, J. C., Shin, L. M., Barrett, L. F., Rauch, S. L., & Wright, C. I. (2008). Amygdala and fusiform gyrus temporal dynamics: Responses to negative facial expressions. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 44. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-9-44 Google Scholar
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism. Child Development, 74, 11081122.Google Scholar
Crosby, J. R., Monin, B., & Richardson, D. (2008). Where do we look during potentially offensive behavior? Psychological Science, 19, 226228. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02072.x Google Scholar
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519526.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). The British Picture Vocabulary Scale (2nd ed.). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Jane Webb, S., Dawson, G., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 189194. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsabbagh, M., Mercure, E., Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Pasco, G., Charman, T., et al. (2012). Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Current Biology, 22, 15.Google Scholar
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47, 248257.Google Scholar
Gilchrist, I. D., Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1996). Grouping and extinction: Evidence for low-level modulation of visual selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 12231249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 313.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns prererential tracking of facelike stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 121.Google Scholar
Jones, C. R. G., Happé, F., Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Marsden, A. J. S., Tregay, J., et al. (2009). Auditory discrimination and auditory sensory behaviours in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 28502858. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.015 Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Preaching to the converted? From constructivism to neuroconstructivism. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 99102. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00086.x Google Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., & Volkmar, F. R. (Eds.). (2005). The enactive mind—From actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809816.Google Scholar
Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Kanai, C., Kurita, H., Osada, H., & Shimizu, K. (2004). A comparison of cognitive profiles on WISC-III between Asperger's disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Seishin Igaku (Clinical Psychiatry), 46, 645647.Google Scholar
Kuhn, G., Benson, V., Fletcher-Watson, S., Kovshoff, H., McCormick, C. A., Kirkby, J., et al. (2010). Eye movements affirm: Automatic overt gaze and arrow cueing for typical adults and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Experimental Brain Research, 201, 155165. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2019-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2001). Early visual experience and face processing. Nature, 410, 890–890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pecini, C., Cesari, A., Paziente, A., et al. (2005). How young children treat objects and people: An empirical study of the first year of life in autism. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 35, 383396.Google Scholar
Miall, R. C., Gowen, E., & Tchalenko, J. (2009). Drawing cartoon faces—A functional imaging study of the cognitive neuroscience of drawing. Cortex, 45, 394406. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.10.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mondloch, C. J., Le Grand, R., Maurer, D., Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. (2003). Early visual experience is necessary for the development of some—but not all—aspects of face processing. In Pascalis, O. & Slater, A. (Eds.), The development of face processing in infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives (pp. 99117): Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164181.Google Scholar
Mottron, L. (2004). Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Current practices, instrument biases, and recommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 1927.Google Scholar
Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In Burack, J. A., Charman, T., Yirmiya, N., & Zelazo, P. R. (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 2743. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7 Google Scholar
Moulson, M. C., Westerlund, A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). The effects of early experience on face recognition: An event-related potential study of institutionalized children in Romania. Child Development, 80, 10391056. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01315.x Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., de Haan, M., & Thomas, K. M. (2006). Neuroscience of cognitive development: The role of experience and the developing brain. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Brock, J., Cragg, L., Einav, S., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2009). Eye-movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 50, 834842.Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 13211323.Google Scholar
Pelphrey, K., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249261.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1941). Standardization of progressive matrices, 1938. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 19, 137150.Google Scholar
Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: Visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 238248. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2011.12.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ristic, J., Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (2002). Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 507513.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Garnham, W., & Rideout, P. (2001). Social understanding in autism: Eye gaze as a measure of core insights. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 10831094.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Andersen-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. (1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following severe early global privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 537549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 445458.Google Scholar
Sigman, M. D., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 647656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skuse, D., Warrington, R., Bishop, D., Chowdhury, U., Lau, J., Mandy, W., et al. (2004). The Developmental, Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview (3di): A novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 548558.Google Scholar
Sparrow, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1985). Diagnostic uses of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 10, 215225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stagg, S. D., Davis, R., & Heaton, P. (2013). Associations between language development and skin conductance responses to faces and eye gaze in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 23032311. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1780-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Duku, E., Vaccarella, L., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bennett, T., et al. (2009). Similar developmental trajectories in autism and Asperger syndrome: From early childhood to adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 14591467. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02123.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tadić, V., Pring, L., & Dale, N. (2010). Are language and social communication intact in children with congenital visual impairment at school age? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 696705. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02200.x Google Scholar
van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Camfferman, G., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2002). Looking at images with human figures: Comparison between autistic and normal children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 6975.Google Scholar
Volkmar, F. R., & Mayes, L. C. (1990). Gaze behavior in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 6169.Google Scholar
Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Buitelaar, J. K., Weijnen, F. G., & van Engeland, H. (1998). Timing of social gaze behaviour in children with a pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 199210.Google Scholar
Wishart, J. G., Cebula, K. R., Willis, D. S., & Pitcairn, T. K. (2007). Understanding of facial expressions of emotion by children with intellectual disabilities of differing aetiology. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 551563.Google Scholar