Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T04:55:20.928Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Morphosyntactic knowledge of clitics by Portuguese heritage bilinguals*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2014

ESTHER RINKE*
Affiliation:
Goethe-University Frankfurt, Germany
CRISTINA FLORES
Affiliation:
University of Minho, Portugal
*
Address for correspondence: Prof. Dr. Esther Rinke, Goethe Universität Frankfurt, Institut für romanische Sprachen und Literaturen, Grüneburgplatz 1, 60629 FrankfurtEsther.rinke@em.uni-frankfurt.de

Abstract

This paper focuses on the linguistic competence of adult Portuguese–German bilinguals in their heritage language, European Portuguese (EP), which they acquired at home in early childhood in the context of German as the majority language. Based on a grammaticality judgment test, we investigate their morphosyntactic knowledge of clitics. The central questions are whether possible deviations from native monolinguals may be traced back to a) lack of contact with the formal register; b) reduced input after preschool age; and c) cross-linguistic influence. The results reveal qualitative differences between the heritage speakers and a group of monolingual controls in almost all test conditions. We conclude that although the linguistic knowledge of the heritage bilinguals investigated in this study differs from that of monolinguals, it is not “deficient” but “different” and “innovative”, because it is primarily based on the spoken variety of the language and because it promotes linguistic changes which are inherent in the speech of native monolinguals.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by a Portuguese–German cooperation grant, funded by the DAAD (German Academic Exchange Service) and the CRUP (Council of Portuguese Universities) [grant numbers 50097448 and A-18/10 AI-A/09]. We would like to thank Pilar Barbosa and Tanja Kupisch for their help in preparing the tests.

References

Andersen, R. W. (1982). Determining the linguistic attributes of language attrition. In Lambert, R. D. & Freed, B. F. (eds.), The loss of language skills, pp. 83118. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Au, T., Knightly, L., Jun, S., & Oh, J. (2002). Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science, 13, 238243.Google Scholar
Barbosa, P. (1996). Clitic placement in European Portuguese and the position of subjects. In Halpern, A. & Zwicky, A. (eds.), Approaching second: Second position clitics and related phenomena, pp. 140. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Barbosa, P. (2008). Clíticos, deslocação local e linearização cíclica. Diacrítica. Série Ciências da Linguagem, 22, 131156.Google Scholar
Bleam, T. (1999). Leísta Spanish and the syntax of clitic doubling. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Bolger, P. A., & Zapata, G. C. (2011). Psycholinguistic approaches to language processing in heritage speakers. Heritage Language Journal, 8, 129.Google Scholar
Cardinaletti, A., & Starke, M. (1999). The typology of structural deficiency: A case study of the three classes of pronouns. In Riemsdijk, H. (ed.), Clitics in the languages of Europe, pp. 145–123. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Costa, J., & Lobo, M. (2007). Clitic omission, null objects or both in the acquisition of European Portuguese? In Baauw, S., Drijkoningen, F., & Pinto, M. (eds.), Romance languages and linguistic theory 2005, pp. 5971. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Costa, J., & Lobo, M. (2011). Objeto nulo na aquisição do português europeu: Pro ou variável? In Costa, A., Falé, I. & Barbosa, P. (eds.), Textos Selecionados do XXVI Encontro da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, pp. 197207. Lisbon: APL.Google Scholar
Costa, J., Fiéis, A., & Lobo, M. (in press). Input variability and late acquisition: Clitic misplacement in European Portuguese. Lingua.Google Scholar
Déchaine, R.-M., & Wiltschko, M. (2002). Decomposing pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 33, 409422.Google Scholar
Duarte, I. (1987). A Construção de Topicalização na Gramática do Português: Regência, Ligação e Condições sobre Movimento. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Lisbon.Google Scholar
Duarte, I. (1996). A topicalização em português europeu: Uma análise comparativa. In Duarte, I. & Leiria, I. (eds.), Actas do congresso internacional sobre o português, pp. 327358. Lisbon: APL/Colibri.Google Scholar
Duarte, I., Matos, G., & Faria, I. H. (1995). Specificity of European Portuguese clitics in Romance. In Faria, I. H. & Freitas, M. J. (eds.), Studies on the acquisition of Portuguese, pp. 129154. Lisbon: APL/Colibri.Google Scholar
Erlam, R. (2013). Effects of instruction on learners’ acquisition of metalinguistic knowledge. In Roehr, K. & Ganem-Gutierre, G. A. (eds.), The metalinguistic dimension in instructed second language learning, pp.7194. New York/London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Fernández Soriano, O. (1999). El pronombre personal. Formas y distribuciones. Pronombres átonos y tónicos. In Bosque, I. & Delmonte, V. (eds.), Gramática descriptiva de la lengua española, 1. Sintaxis básica de las clases de palabras, pp. 12091273. Madrid: Espasa.Google Scholar
Flores, C. (2010). The effect of age on language attrition: Evidence from bilingual returnees. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 533546.Google Scholar
Flores, C. (2012). Differential effects of language attrition in the domains of verb placement and object expression. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 550567.Google Scholar
Flores, C., & Barbosa, P. (2012). When reduced input leads to delayed acquisition: A study on the acquisition of clitic placement by Portuguese heritage speakers. The International Journal of Bilingualism, OnlineFirst.Google Scholar
Franco, Jon (2000). Agreement as a continuum: The case of Spanish pronominal clitics. In Beukema, F. & den Dikken, M. (eds.), Clitic phenomena in European languages, pp. 147189. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frota, S., & Vigário, M. (2001). On the correlates of rhythmic distinctions: the European/Brazilian Portuguese case. Probus, 13, 247275.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2007). Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: Constructivist account of morpho-syntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 224247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 213237.Google Scholar
Genesee, F., & Nicoladis, E. (2006). Bilingual acquisition. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (eds.), Handbook of language development, pp. 324342. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Grolla, E. (2006). The Acquisition of A- and A'-bound pronouns in Brazilian Portuguese. In Torrens, V. & Escobar, L. (eds.), The acquisition of syntax in Romance languages, pp. 227250. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1978). Language as social semiotic: The social interpretation of language and meaning. Edward Arnold: London.Google Scholar
Kaufman, D., & Aronoff, M. (1991). Morphological disintegration and reconstruction in first language attrition. In Seliger, H. W. & Vago, R. M. (eds.), First language attrition, pp. 175188. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keating, G., VanPatten, B., & Jegerski, J. (2011). Who was walking on the beach? Anaphora resolution in Spanish heritage speakers and adult second language learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 193221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J.-H., Montrul, S., & Yoon, J. (2009). Binding interpretation of anaphors in Korean heritage speakers. Language Acquisition, 16, 335.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T. (2012). Specific and generic subjects in the Italian of German–Italian simultaneous bilinguals and L2 learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 736756.Google Scholar
Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). The development of reading in children who speak English as a second language. Developmental Psychology, 39, 10051119.Google Scholar
Magro, C. (2007). Clíticos: Variações sobre o tema. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Lisbon.Google Scholar
Martins, A. M. (1994). Cliticos na Història do Português, Ph.D. dissertation, University of Lisbon.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2001). The simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: Early differentiation and subsequent development of grammars. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition, pp. 1141. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2011). First and second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morpho-syntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 118.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2010a). Dominant language transfer in adult second language learners and heritage speakers. Second Language Research, 26, 293327.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2010b). How similar are adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers? Spanish clitics and word order. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 167207.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Polinsky, M. (2011). Why not heritage speakers? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 5862.Google Scholar
Müller, N., & Hulk, A. (2001). Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Grady, W., Lee, M., & Choo, M. (2001). The acquisition of relative clauses by heritage and nonheritage learners of Korean as a second language: A comparative study. Journal of Korean Language Education, 12, 283294.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (2002). Balancing interpretations regarding effects of bilingualism: Empirical outcomes and theoretical possibilities. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children, pp. 281292. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D., & Rothman, J. (2012). The (il)logical problem of heritage speaker bilingualism and incomplete acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 33, 450455.Google Scholar
Pires, A., & Rothman, J. (2009). Disentangling sources of incomplete acquisition: An explanation for competence divergence across heritage grammars. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13, 211238.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2006). Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14, 191262.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2008). Russian gender under incomplete acquisition. Heritage Language Journal, 6, 4071.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2011). Reanalysis in adult heritage language. New evidence in support of attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 305328.Google Scholar
Raposo, E. P. (1986). On the null object construction in European Portuguese. In Jaeggli, O. & Silva-Corvalán, C. (eds.), Studies in Romance linguistics, pp. 373390. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Raposo, E. P. (2000). Clitic positions and verb movement. In Costa, J. (ed.), Portuguese syntax: New comparative studies, pp. 266297. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2007). Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 11, 359389.Google Scholar
Santos, A. L., & Flores, C. (2012). Comparing heritage speakers and late L2-learners of European Portuguese: Verb movement, VP ellipsis and adverb placement. Paper presented at the Workshop on Crosslinguistic Influence in Non-Native Language Acquisition, Faculdade de Ciências Sociais e Humanas da Universidade Nova de Lisboa, Lisbon, June 29–30, 2012.Google Scholar
Schlyter, S. (1993). The weaker language in bilingual Swedish-French children. In Hyltenstam, K. & Vilberg, A. (eds.), Progression and regression in language, pp. 289308. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Seliger, H. W. (1989). Deterioration and creativity in childhood bilingualism. In Hylstenstam, K. & Obler, L. K. (eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan, pp. 173184. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Senra, T. (2011). Sprachkompetenz der 2. Generation portugiesischer Migranten in Hamburg: unvollständiger Erwerb? Master's thesis, University of Minho, Portugal.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1994). Language contact and change. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2004). Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax–discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 143–45.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2011). Pinning down the concept of interface in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 133.Google Scholar
Suñer, M. (1988). The role of agreement in clitic-doubled constructions. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 6, 391434.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Filiaci, F. (2004). First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 257277.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86110.Google Scholar
Wexler, K., Gavarró, A., & Torrens, V. (2004). Feature checking and object clitic omission in child Catalan and Spanish. In Bok-Bennema, R., Hollebrandse, B., Kampers-Manhe, B. & Sleeman, P. (eds.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2002: Selected papers from ‘Going Romance’, Groningen, 28–30 November 2002, pp. 253269. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar