Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T11:39:27.891Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Production and perception of listener-oriented clear speech in child language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2013

KRISTEN SYRETT*
Affiliation:
Rutgers University – New Brunswick
SHIGETO KAWAHARA
Affiliation:
Keio University, Japan
*
Address for correspondence: Kristen Syrett, Department of Linguistics and Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey – New Brunswick, 18 Seminary Place, New Brunswick, NJ 08901. e-mail: k-syrett@ruccs.rutgers.edu

Abstract

In this paper, we ask whether children are sensitive to the needs of their interlocutor, and, if so, whether they – like adults – modify acoustic characteristics of their speech as part of a communicative goal. In a production task, preschoolers participated in a word learning task that favored the use of clear speech. Children produced vowels that were longer, more intense, more dispersed in the vowel space, and had a more expanded F0 range than normal speech. Two perception studies with adults showed that these acoustic differences were perceptible and were used to distinguish normal and clear speech styles. We conclude that preschoolers are sensitive to aspects of the speaker–hearer relationship calling upon them to modify their speech in ways that benefit their listener.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was made possible from funding provided by NIH grant HD-057699 to K.S., a postdoctoral fellowship from the Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science to K.S., and Rutgers startup grants to K.S. and S. K. We received invaluable research assistance from Lara Greenberg, Melanie Kelliher, Sarah Korostoff, Michelle Marron, and Georgia Simon, and are grateful for participation from the children and staff at Pennington Presbyterian Nursery School, Pine Grove Cooperative Nursery School, and the Douglass Psychology Child Study Center. Finally, this work benefited from comments from two anonymous reviewers, discussions with the audiences at RUMMIT 2010, LSA 2011, the Rutgers Psycholinguistics and Phonetics lab groups, and suggestions on data analysis from Mafuyu Kitahara, Mary Beckman, Britta Lintfert, and Jalal-eddin Al-Tamimi.

References

REFERENCES

Andersen, E. S. (1992). Speaking with style. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Andersen, E. S. (1996). A cross-cultural study of children's register knowledge. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A. & Guo, J. (eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 125–42). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Andersen, E. S., Brizuela, M., DePuy, B. & Gonnerman, L. (1999). Cross-linguistic evidence of the early acquisition of discourse markers as register variables. Journal of Pragmatics 31, 1339–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, A. (1984). Language style as audience design. Language in Society 13, 145204.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2007). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [computer program]. Retrieved from <http://www.praat.org/>..>Google Scholar
Bradlow, A., Torretta, G. & Pisoni, D. (1996). Intelligibility of normal speech I: global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech Communication 20, 255–72.Google Scholar
Cruttenden, A. (1994). Phonetic and prosodic aspects of baby talk. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 135–52). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 28, 125–27. Searchable database: <http://www.sci.sdsu.edu/lexical/>.Google Scholar
Davis, B. & Lindblom, B. (2001). Phonetic variability in baby talk and development of vowel categories. In Lacerda, F., von Hofsten, C. & Heimann, M. (eds.), Emerging cognitive abilities in early infancy (pp. 135–71). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ebeling, K. S. & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Children's use of context in interpreting big and little. Child Development 65, 1178–92.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, K. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (pp. 219–36). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1992). Meaningful melodies in mothers’ speech to infants. In Papoušek, H., Jürgens, U. & Papoušek, M. (eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 262–82). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (2000). Speech to infants as hyperspeech: knowledge-driven processes in early word recognition. Phonetica 57, 242–54.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27, 209–21.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20, 104–13.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., Boysson-Bardies, D. D. & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 16, 477501.Google Scholar
Fisher, C. & Tokura, H. (1995). The given–new contract in speech to infants. Journal of Memory and Language 34, 287310.Google Scholar
Fisher, C. & Tokura, H. (1996). Acoustic cues to grammatical structure in infant-directed speech: cross-linguistic evidence. Child Development 67, 3192–218.Google Scholar
Foulkes, P., Docherty, G. & Watt, D. (2005). Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language 81, 177206.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. & Ebeling, K. S. (1989). Children's use of nonegocentric standards in judgments of functional size. Child Development 60, 920–32.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction. Child Development 59, 2637.Google Scholar
Igarashi, Y. & Mazuka, R. (2008). Exaggerated prosody in infant-directed speech? Intonational phonological analysis of Japanese infant-directed speech. In Chan, H., Jacob, H., and Kapia, E. (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 177–88). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kitahara, M., Nishikawa, K., Igarashi, Y., Shinya, T. & Mazuka, R. (2009). Characteristics of pitch accents in infant-directed speech. Presentation at the Workshop on Prosody and Information Structure, Shiga, Japan.Google Scholar
Krause, J. & Braida, L. (2004). Acoustic properties of naturally produced clear-speech normal speaking rates. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115, 362–78.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P., Andruski, J., Chistovich, I., Chistovich, L., Kozhevnikova, E., Ryskina, V., Stolyarova, E., Sundberg, U. & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277, 684–86.Google Scholar
Lee, S. & Davis, B. L. (2009). Segmental distribution patterns of English infant- and adult-directed speech. Journal of Child Language 37, 767–91.Google Scholar
Lee, S., Davis, B. L. & MacNeilage, P. F. (2008). Segmental properties of input to infants: a study of Korean. Journal of Child Language 35, 591617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review 94, 412–26.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H&H theory. In Hardcastle, W. J. & Marchal, A. (eds.), Speech production and speech modelling (pp. 403–39). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Liu, H. M., Kuhl, P. & Tsao, F. M. (2003). An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science 6, F1F10.Google Scholar
Macmillan, N. A. & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: a user's guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McFarland, D., Jurafsky, D. & Rawlings, C. M. (2013). Making the connection: social bonding in courtship situations. American Journal of Sociology 118, 1596–649.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds.), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (pp. 109149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308, 255–58.Google Scholar
Pardo, J. (2012). Reflections on phonetic convergence: speech perception does not mirror speech production. Language and Linguistics Compass 6, 743–67.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R. & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: the case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 5, 125–37.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1951). The child's conception of the world. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27, 169225.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2006). Alignment as the basis for successful communication. Research on Language and Computation 4, 203–28.Google Scholar
Redford, M. & Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. (2009). The development of distinct speaking styles in preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 52, 1434–48.Google Scholar
Rubio-Fernandez, P. & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. Psychological Science 24, 2733.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (5, Serial No. 152).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M. & Gelman, R. (1977). Beyond syntax: the influence of conversational constraints on speech modification. In Ferguson, C. & Snow, C. (eds.), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (pp. 189–98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shockey, L. & Bond, Z. S. (1980). Phonological processes in speech addressed to children. Phonetica 37, 267–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smiljanić, R. & Bradlow, A. (2009). Speaking and hearing clearly: talker and listener factors in speaking style changes. Language and Linguistics Compass 3, 236–64.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development 43, 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180–93). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Surian, L. & Leslie, A. (1999). Competence and performance in false belief understanding: a comparison of autistic and normal 3-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 17, 141–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, L. & Leonard, L. (1994). Duration of function-word vowels in mothers’ speech to young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 1394–405.Google Scholar
Syrett, K., Kennedy, C. & Lidz, J. (2010). Meaning and context in children's understanding of gradable adjectives. Journal of Semantics 27, 135.Google Scholar
Uchanski, R. M. (2005). Clear speech. In Pisoni, D. B. and Remez, R. (eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 207–35). Malden, MA/Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wagner, L., Greene-Havas, M. & Gillespie, R. (2010). Development in children's comprehension of linguistic register. Child Development 81, 1678–86.Google Scholar
Wassink, A. B., Wright, R. & Franklin, A. (2007). Intraspeaker variability in vowel production: an investigation of motherese, hyperspeech, and Lombard speech in Jamaican speakers. Journal of Phonetics 35, 363–79.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. D. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: machine readable dictionary, Version 2. Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 20, 611. Searchable database: <http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/MRCDataBase/uwa_mrc.htm>.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–28.Google Scholar
Wonnacott, E. & Watson, D. (2008). Acoustic emphasis in four year olds. Cognition 107, 1092–101.Google Scholar