Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T14:46:01.944Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning a generative syntax from transparent syntactic atoms in the linguistic input*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2013

ANAT NINIO*
Affiliation:
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
*
Address for correspondence: Anat Ninio, Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem 91905, Israel. e-mail: Anat.Ninio@huji.ac.il.

Abstract

We examined parents' two-word utterances expressing core syntactic relations in order to test the hypothesis that they may enable children to derive the atoms of hierarchical syntax, namely, the asymmetrical Merge/Dependency relation between pairs of words, and, in addition, to identify variables serving generative syntactic rules. Using a large English-language parental corpus, we located all two-word utterances containing a verb and its subject, object, or indirect object. Analysis showed that parental two-word sentences contain transparent information on the binary dependency/merge relation responsible for syntactic connectivity. The syntactic atoms modelled in the two-word input contain natural variables for dependents, making generalization to other contexts an immediate possibility. In a second study, a large sample of children were found to use the same verbs in the great majority of their early sentences expressing the same core grammatical relations. The results support a learning model according to which children learn the basics of syntax from parental two-word sentences.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Construction of the speech corpora and syntactic annotation were supported under Grant 200900206 to Anat Ninio by the Spencer Foundation. I am grateful to the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, for having me as a Visiting Scholar in the Department of Linguistics and the Institute for Research in Cognitive Science between August 2011 and July 2012, and for making it possible for me to work on this manuscript in a nourishing environment. Thanks are due to Patricia Brooks, Jerome Bruner, Adele Goldberg, Uri Hershberg, Daniel Kahneman, Tamar Keren-Portnoy, Lorraine McCune, Shira Ninio, Michael Tomasello, and two anonymous reviewers for insightful and constructive comments on previous versions of this manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Allerton, D. J. (1982). Valency and the English verb. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Andrews, A. (1985). The major functions of the noun phrase. In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description 1: clause structure, 62154. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1976). Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In Morehead, D. & Morehead, A. (eds), Directions in normal and deficient child language, 99179. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1990). Mapping thematic roles onto syntactic functions: Are children helped by innate linking rules? Linguistics 28, 1253–89.Google Scholar
Brooks, P. & Kempe, V. (2012). Language development. London: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2000). New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cormack, A. & Smith, N. (2001). Don't move! University College London Working Papers in Linguistics 13, 215–41.Google Scholar
Donnellan, K. (1971). Reference and definite descriptions. In Steinberg, D. D. & Jakobovits, L. A. (eds), Semantics: an interdisciplinary reader in philosophy, linguistics, and psychology, 100114. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dowty, D. R. (1991). Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 67, 547619.Google Scholar
Elbers, L. (1990). The synchronic relation between two- and three-word sentences. Paper presented at the Fifth International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Budapest, Hungary.Google Scholar
Foley, W. A. & Van Valin, R. D. (1985). Information packaging in the clause. In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description 1, 282364. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Francis, W. N. & Kučera, H. (1979). Brown corpus manual of information to accompany a standard corpus of present-day American English, revised and amplified. Providence, RI: Brown University, Department of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (1997). Grammatical relations: an introduction. In Givón, T. (ed.), Grammatical relations: a functionalist perspective, 184. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1976). Replies and responses. Language in Society 5, 257313.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: a Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2005). Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Pointing sets the stage for learning language – and creating language. Child Development 78, 741–45.Google Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.Google Scholar
Green, G. M. (1997). Modelling grammar growth: Universal Grammar without innate principles or parameters. Paper presented at GALA97 Conference on Language Acquisition, Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298, 1569–79.Google Scholar
Hill, J. C. (1984). Combining two-term relations: evidence in support of flat structure. Journal of Child Language 11, 673–78.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American 203, 8997.Google Scholar
Hudson, R. (1990). English Word Grammar. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. S. (1997). The architecture of the language faculty. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Treisman, A. & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology 24, 175219.Google Scholar
Kaplan, D. (1989). Demonstratives. In Almog, J., Pezrry, J. & Wettstein, H. (eds), Themes from Kaplan, 481563. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R. & Bresnan, J. (1982). Lexical-Functional Grammar: a formal system for grammatical representation. In Bresnan, J. (ed.), The mental representation of grammatical relations, 173281. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. L. & Comrie, B. (1977). NP accessibility and Universal Grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 8, 63100.Google Scholar
Lee, J. N. & Naigles, L. R. (2005). Input to verb learning in Mandarin Chinese: a role for syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology 41, 529–40.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2006). Deixis. In Horn, L. R. & Ward, G. L. (eds), The handbook of pragmatics, 9781020. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1968). Introduction to theoretical linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. Psychological Review 79, 114.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development 1: syntax and semantics, 73136. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mueller-Gathercole, V. C. (ed.) (2009). Routes to language: studies in honor of Melissa Bowerman. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1994). Predicting the order of acquisition of three-word constructions by the complexity of their dependency structure. First Language 14, 119–52.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (2006). Language and the learning curve: a new theory of syntactic development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. (2011). Syntactic development, its input and output. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. S. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language 5, 115.Google Scholar
O'Grady, W. (2005). Syntactic carpentry: an emergentist approach to syntax. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Osborne, T., Putnam, M. & Gross, T. (2011). Bare phrase structure, label-less trees, and specifier-less syntax: Is Minimalism becoming a dependency grammar? Linguistic Review 28, 315–64.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1865/1982). Logic of the sciences. In Fisch, M., Kloesel, C., Moore, E., Houser, N., et al. (eds), The writings of Charles S. Peirce: a chronological edition 1, 322336. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Peters, S. (1972). The projection problem: How is a grammar to be selected? In Peters, S. (ed.), Goals of linguistic theory, 171–88. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Pine, J. M. & Lieven, E. V. M. (1993). Reanalyzing rote-learned phrases: individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech. Journal of Child Language 20, 551–71.Google Scholar
Powers, S. M. (2002). Merge as a basic mechanism of language: evidence from language acquisition. In Witruk, E. & Friederici, A. D. (eds), Basic functions of language, reading and reading disability, 105–17. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1989). The role of location indexes in spatial perception: a sketch of the FINST spatial index model. Cognition 32, 6597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Radford, A. (2000). Children in search of perfection: towards a Minimalist model of acquisition. Essex Research Reports in Linguistics 33, 2132.Google Scholar
Rispoli, M. (1994). Structural dependency and the acquisition of grammatical relations. In Levy, Y. (ed.), Other children, other languages: issues in the theory of language acquisition, 265302. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (1986). Constituency or dependency in the units of language acquisition? An approach to describing the learner's analysis of formulae. Lingvisticae Investigationes, International Journal of French Linguistics and General Linguistics 10, 417–37.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. (1995). On the semantics of the object. In Aarts, B. & Meyer, C. F. (eds), The verb in contemporary English, 5474. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Scholl, B. J. (2001). Objects and attention: the state of the art. Cognition 80, 146.Google Scholar
Sethuraman, N. & Goodman, J. C. (2004). Children's mastery of the transitive construction. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Online proceedings of the 32nd session of the Stanford Child Language Research Forum, 6067. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Available at <http://www-csli.stanford.edu/pubs>.Google Scholar
Tesnière, L. (1959). Éléments de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck.Google Scholar
Van Langendonck, W. (1987). Word Grammar and child grammar. Belgian Journal of Linguistics 2, 109–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Valin, R. D. Jr. (1993). A synopsis of Role and Reference Grammar. In Van Valin, R. D. (ed.), Advances in Role and Reference Grammar, 1164. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zamuner, T. S., Gerken, L. A. & Hammond, M. (2005). The acquisition of phonology based on input: a closer look at the relation of cross-linguistic and child language data. Lingua 115, 1403–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar