Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T18:20:46.964Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

FROM SEEING ADVERBS TO SEEING VERBAL MORPHOLOGY

Language Experience and Adult Acquisition of L2 Tense

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 June 2013

Nuria Sagarra*
Affiliation:
Rutgers University
Nick C. Ellis
Affiliation:
University of Michigan
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nuria Sagarra, Department of Spanish and Portuguese, Rutgers University, 105 George St., New Brunswick, NJ 08901. E-mail: nuria.sagarra@rutgers.edu

Abstract

Adult learners have persistent difficulty processing second language (L2) inflectional morphology. We investigate associative learning explanations that involve the blocking of later experienced cues by earlier learned ones in the first language (L1; i.e., transfer) and the L2 (i.e., proficiency). Sagarra (2008) and Ellis and Sagarra (2010b) found that, unlike Spanish monolinguals, intermediate English-Spanish learners rely more on salient adverbs than on less salient verb inflections, but it is not clear whether this preference is a result of a default or a L1-based strategy. To address this question, 120 English (poor morphology) and Romanian (rich morphology) learners of Spanish (rich morphology) and 98 English, Romanian, and Spanish monolinguals read sentences in L2 Spanish (or their L1 in the case of the monolinguals) containing adverb-verb and verb-adverb congruencies or incongruencies and chose one of four pictures after each sentence (i.e., two that competed for meaning and two for form). Eye-tracking data revealed significant effects for (a) sensitivity (all participants were sensitive to tense incongruencies), (b) cue location in the sentence (participants spent more time at their preferred cue, regardless of its position), (c) L1 experience (morphologically rich L1 learners and monolinguals looked longer at verbs than morphologically poor L1 learners and monolinguals), and (d) L2 experience (low-proficiency learners read more slowly and regressed longer than high-proficiency learners). We conclude that intermediate and advanced learners are sensitive to tense incongruencies and—like native speakers—tend to rely more heavily on verbs if their L1 is morphologically rich. These findings reinforce theories that support transfer effects such as the unified competition model and the associative learning model but do not contradict Clahsen and Felser’s (2006a) shallow structure hypothesis because the target structure was morphological agreement rather than syntactic agreement.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research was supported by a National Science Foundation Grant BCS-0717557. Special thanks to the Instituto Cervantes de Bucarest, Romania.

References

Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2000). Tense and aspect in second laguage acquisition: Form, meaning, and use. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bordag, D., & Pechmann, T. (2007). Factors influencing L2 gender processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 299314.Google Scholar
Carroll, S. E. (2001). Input and evidence: The raw materials of second language acquisition. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006a). Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 107126.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006b). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Diego Balaguer, R., & López-Barroso, D. (2010). Cognitive and neural mechanisms sustaining rule learning from speech. Language Learning, 60(2), 151187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, R., Klein, W., & Noyau, C. (1995). Acquisition of temporality in a second language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ditman, T., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2009). An investigation of concurrent ERP and self-paced reading methodologies. Psychophysiology, 44, 927935.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. (2010). Uses of eye-tracking data in second language sentence processing research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 30, 149166.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2006). The Associative-Cognitive CREED. In VanPatten, B. & Williams, J. (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An Introduction (pp. 7795). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2007). Blocking and learned attention in language acquisition. CogSci 2007, Proceedings of the Twenty-Ninth Cognitive Science Conference. Nashville, Tennessee, August 1–4, 2007. Retrieved from http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2007/index.htm Google Scholar
Ellis, N., Hafeez, K., Martin, K. I., Chen, L., Boland, J., & Sagarra, N. (2012). An eye-tracking study of learned attention in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics. Advance online publication. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000501 Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2010a). The bounds of adult language acquisition: Blocking and learned attention. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 128.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2010b). Learned attention effects in L2 temporal reference: The first hour and the next eight semesters. Language Learning, 60, 85108.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2011). Blocking and learned attention in language acquisition: A replication and generalization study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 589624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V. (2003). The structure of time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D. (1998). Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 285319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. (2005). Eye-movement recording as a tool for studying syntactic processing in a second language: A review of methodologies and experimental findings. Second Language Research, 21, 175198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giacalone-Ramat, A. (1992). Grammaticalization processes in the area of temporal and modal relations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 297322.Google Scholar
Goldschneider, J. M., & DeKeyser, R. M. (2001). Explaining the “natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition” in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants. Language Learning, 51, 150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2007). Ultimate attainment at the interfaces in second language acquisition: Grammar and processing (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Groningen, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 603634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 57, 133.Google Scholar
Jiang, N., Novokshanova, E., Masuda, K., & Wang, X. (2011). Morphological sensitivity and the acquisition of L2 morphemes. Language Learning, 61, 940967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. (2005). The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language. Second Language Research, 21, 121151.Google Scholar
Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In Campbell, B. A. & Church, R. M. (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 276296). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Kitagawa, Y., & Fodor, J. (2006). Prosodic influence on syntactic judgments. In Fanselow, G., Féry, C., Schlesewsky, M., & Vogel, R. (Eds.), Gradience in grammar: Generative perspectives (pp. 336358). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. (2006, June). Learned attention. Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on Development and Learning, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K., & Blair, N. J. (2000). Blocking and backward blocking involve learned inattention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 636645.Google Scholar
LaBrozzi, R. (2009). Processing of lexical and morphological cues in a study abroad context (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The Pennsylvania State University, University Park.Google Scholar
Lee, J. F. (2002). The incidental acquisition of Spanish future morphology through reading in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 5580.Google Scholar
Lee, J. F., Cadierno, T., Glass, W. R., & VanPatten, B. (1997). The effects of lexical and grammatical cues on processing past temporal reference in second language input. Applied Language Learning, 8, 123.Google Scholar
Leeser, M. (2004). The effects of topic familiarity, mode, and pausing on second language learners’ comprehension and focus on form. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 587615.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In Kroll, J. & de Groot, A. M. B. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 4957). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2011). The logic of the Unified Model. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 85112). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Musumeci, D. (1989). The ability of second language learners to assign tense at the sentence level: A cross-linguistic study (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Park, D., Welsh, R., Marschuetz, C., Gutchess, A., Mikels, J., Polk, T., Noll, D., & Taylor, S. (2003). Working memory for complex scenes: Age differences in frontal and hippocampal activations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 11221134.Google Scholar
Parodi, T., Schwartz, B., & Clahsen, H. (2004). On the L2 acquisition of the morphosyntax of German nominals. Linguistics, 42, 669705.Google Scholar
Peña, M., Bonatti, L. L., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2002). Signal-driven computations in speech processing. Science, 298, 604607.Google Scholar
Pynte, J., & Colonna, S. (2000). Decoupling syntactic parsing from visual inspection: The case of relative clause attachment in French. In Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D., & Pynte, J. (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 529547). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., & Clifton, C. (2002). Language processing. In Medin, D. (Ed.), Stevens handbook of experimental psychology: Memory and cognitive processes (3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 261316). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Rossomondo, A. E. (2003). The role of lexical temporal indicators in the incidental acquisition of the Spanish future tense (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Indiana University, Bloomington.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N. (2008). Working memory and L2 processing of redundant grammatical forms. In Han, Z. (Ed.), Second language processing and instruction: Broadening the scope of inquiry (pp. 142159). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J. (2010). The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 20, 20222039.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J. (2011). Proficiency and animacy effects on L2 gender agreement processes during comprehension. Language Learning, 61, 80116.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Seibert Hanson, A. (2011). Eyetracking methodology: A user’s guide for linguistic research. Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 4, 543555.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (1987). The modular basis of second language acquisition (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Skiba, R., & Dittmar, N. (1992). Pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic constraints and grammaticalization: A longitudinal perspective. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 323349.Google Scholar
Starren, M. (2001). The second time: The acquisition of temporality in Dutch and French as a second language. Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 173204.Google Scholar
Tolentino, L. C., & Tokowicz, N. (2011). Across languages, space, and time: A review of the role of cross-language similarity in L2 (morpho)syntactic processing as revealed by fMRI and ERP methods. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 31125.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 285318.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2007). Input processing in adult second language acquisition. In VanPatten, B. & Williams, J. (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Keating, G. D. (2007, April). Getting tense. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Association of Applied Linguistics, Costa Mesa, CA.Google Scholar
Williams, R. S., & Morris, R. K. (2004). Eye movements, word familiarity, and vocabulary acquisition. In Radach, R., Kennedy, A., & Rayner, K. (Eds.), Eye movements and information processing during reading (pp. 312339). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. P., & Garnsey, S. (2009). Making simple sentences hard: Verb bias effects in simple direct object sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 368392.Google Scholar
Wong, W. (2001). Modality and attention to meaning and form in the input. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 345368.Google Scholar
Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behaviour and the principle of least effort: An introduction to human ecology. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar