Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:28:35.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between listening comprehension of text and sentences in preschoolers: Specific or mediated by lower and higher level components?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2011

ELENA FLORIT
Affiliation:
University of Padova
MAJA ROCH
Affiliation:
University of Padova
M. CHIARA LEVORATO*
Affiliation:
University of Padova
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE M. Chiara Levorato, Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e dei Processi di Socializzazione, Via Venezia 8, Padova 35133, Italy. E-mail: chiara.levorato@unipd.it

Abstract

Two studies explored the relation between listening comprehension of text and listening comprehension of sentences in preschoolers aged 4 to 5 years, 11 months. The first study analyzed this relationship taking into account the role of lower level components, namely, word knowledge and verbal working memory, as possible mediators. These components specifically accounted for listening text comprehension, whereas sentence comprehension did not. Given that sentences forming a text are not processed in isolation but in context, the second study explored the role of the ability to use linguistic context, a higher level component, in listening comprehension of text and sentences. Listening sentence comprehension was facilitated by the use of context, which accounted for individual differences in listening text comprehension. Overall, results showed that listening text comprehension is related to lower level as well as higher level components, whereas listening sentence comprehension does not play a specific role.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 16891716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. (1983). Test for the Reception of Grammar. Unpublished manuscript, University of Manchester, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Cain, K. (2006). Individual differences in children's memory and reading comprehension: An investigation of semantic and inhibitory deficits. Memory, 14, 553569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (2004). Reading comprehension difficulties. In Nunes, T. & Bryant, P. (Eds.), Handbook of children's literacy (pp. 313338). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 683696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (2007). Reading comprehension difficulties: Correlates, causes and consequences. In Cain, K. & Oakhill, J. V. (Eds.), Children's comprehension problems in oral and written language: A cognitive perspective (pp. 340). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Bryant, P. E. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 671681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Lemmon, K. (2005). The relation between children's reading comprehension level and their comprehension of idioms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 6587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., Patson, N., & Andrews, L. (2005). Age- and ability-related differences in young readers' use of conjunctions. Journal of Child Language, 32, 877892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., & Towse, A. S. (2008). To get the wrong end of the stick: Reasons for poor idiom understanding in children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 15381549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., Towse, A. S., & Knight, R. S. (2009). The development of idiom comprehension: An investigation of semantic and contextual processing skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 98108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., & Janosky, J. (1997). Reducing bias in language assessment: Processing-dependent measure. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 519525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 246251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised: Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Florit, E., Roch, M., Altoè, G., & Levorato, M. C. (2009). Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 935951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1994). Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goff, D., Pratt, C., & Ong, B. (2005). The relations between children's reading comprehension, working memory, language skills and components of reading decoding in a normal sample. Reading and Writing, 18, 583616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (2001). A new tool for measuring and understanding individual differences in the component process of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 103128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., White, M. J., & van den Broek, P. (2008). Children's inference generation across different media. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of decoding and oral language skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 765778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension: The CI perspective. Discourse Processes, 39, 125128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Normand, M. T., Parisse, C., & Cohen, H. (2008). Lexical diversity and productivity in French preschoolers: Developmental, gender and sociocultural factors. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22, 4758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levorato, M. C., & Cacciari, C. (1992). Children's comprehension and production of idioms: The role of context and familiarity. Journal of Child Language, 19, 415433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levorato, M. C., & Cacciari, C. (1999). Idiom comprehension in children: Are the effects of semantic analysability and context separable? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 5166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levorato, M. C., Nesi, B., & Cacciari, C. (2004). Reading comprehension and understanding idioms: A developmental study. Brain and Language, 202, 416.Google Scholar
Levorato, M. C., & Roch, M. (2007). Valutare la comprensione del testo orale: Il TOR 3–8 [Evaluation of listening comprehension: The TOR 3–8]. Florence: Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Levorato, M. C. & Roch, M., & Nesi, B. (2007). A longitudinal study of idiom and text comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 34, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marconi, L., Ott, M., Pesenti, E., Ratti, D., & Tarella, M. (1994). Lessico elementare. Dati scolastici sull'italiano scritto e parlato dai bambini delle elementari [Basic vocabulary. Data on written and spoken Italian of children at the elementary school]. Bologna: Il Mulino.Google Scholar
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nash, H., & Snowling, M. (2006). Teaching new words to children with poor existing vocabulary knowledge: A controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 335354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 199211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S. H., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 51, 10311039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence for dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69, 9961011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesi, B., Levorato, M. C., Roch, M., & Cacciari, C. (2006). To break the . . . embarrassment: Text comprehension skills and figurative competence in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders. European Psychologist, 11, 128136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2007). Introduction to comprehension development. In Cain, K. & Oakhill, J. V. (Eds.), Children's comprehension problems in oral and written language: A cognitive perspective (pp. 4173). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence for component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. Journal of Educational Research, 95, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rustioni, M. L. D., & Associazione “La Nostra Famiglia” (1994). Prove di Valutazione della Comprensione Linguistica [Test for the Evaluation of Linguistic Comprehension]. Florence: Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Seigneuric, A., Ehrlich, M. F., Oakhill, J. V., & Yuill, N. (2000). Working memory resources and children's reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 13, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stella, G., Pizzoli, C., & Tressoldi, P. E. (2000). Peabody—Test di Vocabolario Recettivo—PPVT-R. Turin: Omega.Google Scholar
Storch, S., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stothard, S. E., & Hulme, C. (1992). Reading comprehension difficulties in children: The role of language comprehension and working memory skills. Reading and Writing, 4, 245256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stothard, S. E., & Hulme, C. (1996). A comparison of reading comprehension and decoding difficulties in children. In Cornoldi, C. & Oakhill, J. (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties. Processes and intervention (pp. 93112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., Kremer, K., Lynch, J., Butler, J., White, M. J., et al. (2005). Assessment of Comprehension abilities in young children. In Paris, S. G. & Stahl, S. A. (Eds.), Children's reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 107130). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1967). Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. New York: Psychological Corporation. Italian standardization by Orsini, A., & Picone, L. (1996). WPPSI. Contributo alla taratura Italiana [WPPSI. Contribution to Italian standardization]. Florence: Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Willis, C. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory contributions to sentence processing in young children. Memory, 9, 349363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. V. (1991). Children's problems in text comprehension. An experimental investigation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar