Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T17:12:51.635Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phenotype–environment correlations in longitudinal twin models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2013

Christopher R. Beam*
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
Eric Turkheimer
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Christopher R. Beam, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904; E-mail: crb8v@virginia.edu.

Abstract

Gene–environment correlation (rGE) exists both within and between families. Between families, accumulating rGE has been used to explain dramatic changes in phenotypic means over time. The Dickens and Flynn model of increases in cognitive ability over generational time, for example, suggests that small changes in phenotype can lead to subsequent reallocation of environmental resources. This process sets up a reciprocal feedback loop between phenotype and environment, producing accumulating rGE that can cause large changes in the mean of ability, even though ability remains highly heritable in cross-sectional data. We report simulations suggesting that similar processes may operate within twin and sibling pairs. Especially in dizygotic twins and siblings, small differences in phenotype can become associated with reallocations of environmental resources within families. We show that phenotype–environment effects can account for age-related increases in rGE, rapid differentiation of siblings raised together, and widely reported increases in the heritability of behavior during childhood and adolescence.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bartels, M., van den Oord, E. J. C. G., Hudziak, J. J., Rietveld, M. J. H., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2004). Genetic and environmental mechanisms underlying stability and change in problem behaviors at ages 3, 7, 10, and 12. Developmental Psychology, 40, 852867.Google Scholar
Bergen, S. E., Gardner, C. O., & Kendler, K. S. (2007). Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adolescence and young adulthood: A meta-analysis. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 423433.Google Scholar
Bishop, E. G., Cherny, S. S., Corley, R., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2003). Development genetic analysis of general cognitive ability from 1 to 12 years in a sample of adoptees, biological siblings, and twins. Intelligence, 31, 3149.Google Scholar
Davis, O. S. P., Haworth, C. M. A., & Plomin, R. (2009). Dramatic increase in heritability of cognitive development from early to middle childhood: An 8-year longitudinal study of 8700 pairs of twins. Psychological Science, 20, 13011308.Google Scholar
Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox revisited. Psychological Review, 108, 346369.Google Scholar
Dickens, W. T., Turkheimer, E., & Beam, C. (2011). The social dynamics of the expression of genes for cognitive ability. In Kendler, K. S., Jaffee, S. R., & Romer, D. (Eds.), The dynamic genome and mental health: The role of genes and environments in youth development (pp. 103127). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eaves, L. J., Long, J., & Heath, A. C. (1986). A theory of developmental change in quantitative phenotypes applied to cognitive development. Behavior Genetics, 16, 143162.Google Scholar
Haworth, C. M. A., Carnell, S., Meaburn, E. L., Oliver, S. P. D., Plomin, R., & Wardle, J. (2008). Increasing heritability of BMI and stronger associations with the FTO gene over childhood. Obesity, 16, 26632668.Google Scholar
Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, D., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Longitudinal genetic study of verbal and nonverbal IQ from early childhood to young adulthood. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 97114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jinks, J. L., & Fulker, D. W. (1970). Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 311349.Google Scholar
Klump, K. L., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2000). Age differences in genetic and environmental influences on eating attitudes and behaviors in preadolescent and adolescent female twins. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 239251.Google Scholar
McArdle, J. J., & Epstein, D. (1987). Latent growth curves within developmental structural equation models. Child Development, 58, 110133.Google Scholar
McGue, M., & Christensen, K. (2012). Growing old but not growing apart: Twin similarity in the latter half of the lifespan. Behavior Genetics, 42.Google Scholar
Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (1998–2011). Mplus user's guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles: Author.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Spinath, F. M. (2004). Intelligence: Genetics, genes, and genomics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 112129.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype → environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424435.Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E. (2004). Spinach and ice cream: Why social science is so difficult. In DiLalla, L. (Ed.), Behavior genetics principles: Perspectives in development, personality, and psychopathology (pp. 161189). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E., & Gottesman, I. I. (1996). Simulating the dynamics of genes and environment in development. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 667677.Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E., & Harden, K. P. (in press). Behavior genetic research methods: Testing quasi-causal hypotheses using multivariate twin data. In Reis, H. T. & Judd, C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. S. (1983). The Louisville Twin Study: Developmental synchronies in behavior. Child Development, 54, 298316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed