Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T09:30:19.043Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why Don't Girls Choose Technological Studies? Adolescents' Stereotypes and Attitudes towards Studies Related to Medicine or Engineering

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Mercedes López-Sáez*
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
Susana Puertas
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Milagros Sáinz
Affiliation:
Universitat Oberta de Catalunya (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mercedes López-Sáez. Departamento de Psicología Social y de las Organizaciones. Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED). Juan del Rosal 10. 28040 Madrid. (Spain). E-mail: mlopez@psi.uned.es

Abstract

Gender differences in choice of studies emerge already in adolescence. Two studies with adolescents are presented, the goal of which is to explore the influence of gender by assessing males and females who choose studies related to Medicine or Engineering. Study 1, correlational (N = 330, mean age 15.9, 56.7% girls), shows that girls who choose technology are more poorly appraised than girls who choose other studies. Study 2 (N = 130; mean age 16.77, 56.2% girls), experimental, measures implicit attitudes (using the IAT) towards males and females from Medicine and Engineering. Implicit attitudes are more favorable towards women if they are studying Medicine and towards men if they study Engineering. The results are analyzed with relation to the percentages of boys and girls in the different fields of study.

Las diferencias de género en elección de estudios aparecen ya en la adolescencia. Se presentan dos estudios con muestras de adolescentes, cuyo objetivo es explorar la influencia del género al evaluar a hombres y mujeres que eligen estudios relacionados con Medicina o con Ingeniería. El estudio 1, correlacional (N = 330; media de edad 15,9), muestra que la chica de tecnología es peor evaluada que la que elige otro tipo de estudios. El estudio 2 (N = 130; media de edad 17,8), experimental, mide actitudes implícitas (utilizando el IAT) hacia hombres y mujeres de Medicina e Ingeniería. Las actitudes implícitas hacia las mujeres son más favorables si pertenecen a Medicina y hacia los hombres si pertenecen a Ingeniería. Los resultados se analizan en relación con las tasas de chicas y chicos en las distintas ramas de estudio.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ajzen, I. (1991). Theory of planed behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-TCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: the case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380393. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.83.2.380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayalon, H. (2003). Women and men go to university: Mathematical background and gender differences in choice of field in higher education. Sex Roles, 48, 277290. doi:10.1023/A:1022829522556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A., Barbaranelly, C., Caprana, C. V., & Pastorelli, C. (2001). Self-efficacy beliefs as shapers of children's aspirations and career trajectories. Child Development, 72, 187206. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banse, R., Seise, J., & Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145160. doi:10.1026%2F%2F0949-3946.48.2.145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barberá, E., Candela, C., & Ramos, A. (2008). Elección de carrera, desarrollo profesional y estereotipos de género [Career selection, professional development and gender stereotypes]. Revista de Psicología Social, 23, 275285. doi:10.1174%2F021347408784135805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Biernat, M. (1991). Gender stereotypes and the relationship between masculinity and femininity. A developmental analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 351365. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.61.3.351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, I. (2002) The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242261. doi:10.1207%2FS15327957PSPR0603_8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breakwell, G. M., Vignoles, V. L., & Robertson, T. (2003). Stereotypes and crossed-category evaluations: The case of gender and science education. British Journal of Education, 94, 437455. doi:10.1348%2F000712603322503024Google ScholarPubMed
Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypes in person perception. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 352). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Centro de Investigación y Documentación Educativa (CIDE), & Instituto de la Mujer (1988). La presencia de las mujeres en el sistema educativo [The presence of women in the educational system]. Madrid: Instituto de la Mujer.Google Scholar
Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, G. A. (2001) On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800814. doi:10.1006%2Fjesp.1999.1418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasgupta, N., Mcghee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Automatic preference for white americans: eliminating the familiarity explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 316328. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.81.5.800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (2005). Attitude research in the 21st century: The current state of knowledge. In Albarracín, D., Jonson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P., (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 743767). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573598. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0033-295X.109.3.573CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior. Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54, 408423. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0003 -066X.54.6.408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In Eckes, T., & Trautner, H. M. (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp.123174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1983). Expectancies, values and academic behaviors. In Spence, J. T., Achievement and achievement motives. Psychological and sociological approaches. (pp. 75146). San Francisco, CA: Freeman and Company.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women's achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135172. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1987.tb00781.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1989). Bringing young women to math and science. In Crawford, M., & Gentry, M., (Eds). Gender and thought (pp. 3658). Nueva York, NY: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women's educational and occupational choices. Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 585609. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb01049.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (2001). Achievement. In Worell, J. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of women and gender: Sex similarities and differences and the impact of society on gender (pp. 4353). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences in participation in physical science and engineering. In Ceci, J. S., & Williams, W. M. (Eds.), Why aren't more women in science? Top researchers debate evidence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11546-016Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., Barber, B., & Jozefowicz, D. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. In Swann, W. B., Langlois, J. H., & Gilbert, L. A. (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society (pp. 153191). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10277-007Google Scholar
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values and goals. Annual Reviews Psychology, 53, 109132. doi:10.1146%2Fannurev.psych.53.100901.135153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elejabeitia, C., & López-Sáez, M. (2003). Trayectorias personales y profesionales de mujeres con estudios tradicionalmente masculinos [Personal and professional trajectories of women enrolled in traditionally masculine studies]. Madrid: CIDE/Instituto de la Mujer.Google Scholar
Fagot, B. I. Rodgers, C. S. Leinbach, M. D. (2000). Theories of gender socialization. In Eckes, T., & Trautner, H. M. (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 6589). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Frome, P. M., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Parents' influence on children's achievement-related perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 435452. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.74.2.435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grañeras, M., Del Olmo, G., Gil, N., García, M., & Boix, M. (2001). Las mujeres en el sistema educativo [Women in the educational system]. Madrid: Instituto de la Mujer.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition. The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.74.6.1464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197216. doi:10.1037%2F0022-3514.85.2.197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannover, B., & Kessels, U. (2002). Challenge the science-stereotype! Der Einfluss von Technikfreizeitkursen auf das Naturwissenschaften-Stereotyp von Schülerinnen und Schülern [The influence of technology-oriented extra-academic courses on male and female students' stereotypes about Natural Sciences]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 341358.Google Scholar
Hannover, B., & Kessels, U. (2004). Self-to-prototype matching as a strategy for making academic choices. Why high school students do not like math and science. Learning and Instruction, 14, 5167. doi:10.1016%2Fj.learninstruc.2003.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausmann, R., Tyson, L. D., & Zahidi, S. (2008). The Global Gender Gap (World Economic Forum Report 2007). Retrieved from World Economic Forum website: http://www.weforum.org/pdf/gendergap/report2008.pdfGoogle Scholar
Instituto de la Mujer (2009). Alumnado universitario matriculado según área de conocimiento [University students registered by subject areas]. Retrieved from: http://www.migualdad.es/mujer/mujeres/cifras/tablas/W128.XLSGoogle Scholar
Instituto Nacional de Estadística (2009) Alumnado. Régimen general. Curso 2005-2006. Educación secundaria. [Students. General enrolment. Course 2005-2006. High School]. Retrieved from: http://www.ine.es/jaxi/tabla.do?path=/t13/p001/e03/a2005-2006/10/&file=amg50014.px&type=pcaxis&L=0Google Scholar
Kessels, U. (2005). Fitting into the stereotype: How gender-stereotyped perceptions of prototypic peers relate to liking for school subjects. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 20, 309323. doi:10.1007%2FBF03173559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessels, U., & Hannover, B. (2008). When being a girl matters less. Accessibility of gender-related self-knowledge in single-sex and coeducational classes. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 273289. doi:10.1348%2F000709907X215938CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessels, U., Rau, M., & Hannover, B. (2006). What goes well with physics? Measuring and altering the image of science. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 761780. doi:10.1348/000709905X59961CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köller, O., Schnabel, K. U., & Bäumert, J. (2001). Does interest matter? The relationship between academic interest and achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 32, 448471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Sáez, M., Puertas, S., & Sáinz, M. (2008, July). Actitudes de los adolescentes hacia Médicas/os e Ingenieras/os. [Attitudes of adolescents towards women and men from Medicine and Engineering] 10th International Interdisciplinary Congress, Women's Worlds, Madrid (Spain).Google Scholar
McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435442. doi:10.1006%2Fjesp.2000.1470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = male, me = female, therefore Math not = me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 4459. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., … Greenwald, A. G. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(26), 1059310597. doi:10.1073%2Fpnas.0809921106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ottaway, S. A., Hayden, D. C., & Oakes, M. A. (2001). Implicit attitudes and racism: effects of word familiarity and frequency on the implicit association test. Social Cognition, 19, 97144. doi:10.1521%2Fsoco.19.2.97.20706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puertas, S., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., & Moya, M. C. (2002). Procesamiento automático y medición implícita de los estereotipos relacionados con el poder [Automatic processing and implicit measure of power related stereotypes]. Revista de Psicología Social, 17, 6984. doi:10.1174%2F021347402753408677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Mcghee, D. E. (2001). Implicit Self-Concept and Evaluative Implicit gender stereotypes: self and Ingroup Share Desirable Traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 11641178. doi:10.1177%2F0146167201279009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellot, D. S., & Schwartz, L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: flexibility and generality of the implicit association test. Social Cognition, 17, 437465. doi:10.1521%2Fsoco.1999.17.4.437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruiz-Ben, E. (2003). Attitudes towards computing among secondary students in Spain: Gender based differences. In Pasero, U., & Gottburgsen, A. (Eds.), Wie natürlich ist Geschlecht? Gender und die Konstruktion von Natur und Technik [How natural is gender? Gender and the construction of Nature and Technology]. (pp. 256269). Rosch-Buch, ScheBlitz: Westdeutscher Verlag.Google Scholar
Sáinz, M., & González, A. (2008). La segunda brecha digital: educación e investigación [The second digital divide: education and research]. In Castaño, C. (Dir.), La segunda brecha digital [The second digital divide] (pp. 221266). Madrid: Ediciones Cátedra.Google Scholar
Stake, J. E., & Nickens, S. D. (2005). Adolescent girls' and boys' science peer relationships and perceptions of the possible self as scientist. Sex Roles, 52, 111. doi:10.1007%2Fs11199-005-1189-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, J. E., Rudman, L. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). Using the Implicit Association Test to investigate attitude-behaviour consistency for stigmatised behavior. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 207230. doi:10.1080%2F0269993004200060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (1996). Egalitarian and traditional families: What do they mean for girls' and boys' achievement in math and science? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 7388. doi:10.1007/BF01537381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, M. J., & White, G. B. (2006). Implicit and Explicit Occupational Gender Stereotypes. Sex Roles, 55, 259266. doi:10.1007%2Fs11199-006-9078-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar