Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T18:48:11.165Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verb inflection in German-learning children with typical and atypical language acquisition: the impact of subsyllabic frequencies*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2012

SUSAN OTT
Affiliation:
Linguistics Department, University of Potsdam, Germany
BARBARA HÖHLE
Affiliation:
Linguistics Department, University of Potsdam, Germany

Abstract

Previous research has shown that high phonotactic frequencies facilitate the production of regularly inflected verbs in English-learning children with specific language impairment (SLI) but not with typical development (TD). We asked whether this finding can be replicated for German, a language with a much more complex inflectional verb paradigm than English. Using an elicitation task, the production of inflected nonce verb forms (3rd person singular with -t suffix) with either high- or low-frequency subsyllables was tested in sixteen German-learning children with SLI (ages 4;1–5;1), sixteen TD-children matched for chronological age (CA) and fourteen TD-children matched for verbal age (VA) (ages 3;0–3;11). The findings revealed that children with SLI, but not CA- or VA-children, showed differential performance between the two types of verbs, producing more inflectional errors when the verb forms resulted in low-frequency subsyllables than when they resulted in high-frequency subsyllables, replicating the results from English-learning children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Address for correspondence: Susan Ott, University of Potsdam, Linguistics Department, Karl-Liebknecht-Str. 24/25, D-14476 Potsdam, Germany. tel: +49 331 977 2934; fax: +49 331 977 2095; e-mail: susott@googlemail.com.

References

REFERENCES

Aichert, I., Marquardt, C. & Ziegler, W. (2005). Frequenzen sublexikalischer Einheiten des Deutschen. CELEX-basierteDatenbanken. Neurolinguistik 19, 5581.Google Scholar
Archer, S. & Curtin, S. (2008). Infant perception of language specific phonotactics. Poster presented at the 26th International Congress of Infant Studies (ICIS), Vancouver.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R. & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX Lexical Database (Release 2) [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Bailey, T. M. & Hahn, U. (2001). Determinants of wordlikeness. Phonotactics or lexical neighborhoods? Journal of Memory and Language 44, 568–91.Google Scholar
Beckman, M. E. & Edwards, J. (2000). Lexical frequency effects on young children's imitative productions. In Broe, M. & Pierrehumbert, J. (eds), Papers in laboratory phonology V. Language acquisition and the lexicon, 208218. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1994). Grammatical errors in specific language impairment. Competence or performance limitations? Applied Psycholinguistics 15, 507550.Google Scholar
Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M. & Rathbun, K. (2005). Mommy and me. Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science 16, 298304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H., Eisenbeiss, S. & Penke, M. (1996). Lexical learning in early syntactic development. In Clahsen, H. (ed.), Generative perspectives on language acquisition, 129–59. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Coady, J. A., Evans, J. L. & Kluender, K. R. (2010a). The role of phonotactic frequency in nonword repetition by children with specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders 45, 494509.Google Scholar
Coady, J. A., Evans, J. L. & Kluender, K. R. (2010b). The role of phonotactic frequency in sentence repetition by children with specific language impairments. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 52, 1401–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D., Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (1989). The grammatical analysis of language disability, 2nd edn.London: Cole & Whurr.Google Scholar
Daugherty, K. & Seidenberg, M. (1992). Rules or connections? The past tense revisited. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 259–64. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Edwards, J. & Beckman, M. E. (2008). Some cross-linguistic evidence for modulation of implicational universals by language-specific frequency effects in phonological development. Language, Learning and Development 4, 122–56.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. (1994). On the acquisition of prosodic structure. Dordrecht: ICG Printing.Google Scholar
Fox, A. V. & Dodd, B. (1999). Der Erwerb des phonologischen Systems in der deutschen Sprache. Sprache, Stimme, Gehör 23, 183–91.Google Scholar
Fox, A. V. & Schoop, S. (2006). Test zur Überprüfung des Grammatikverständnisses (TROG-D). Idstein: Schulz-Kirchner.Google Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.Google Scholar
Grijzenhout, J. & Joppen-Hellwig, S. (2002). The lack of onsets in German child phonology. In Lasser, I. (ed.), The process of language acquisition. Proceedings of GALA 1999, 319–39. Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Hall, T. A. (1992). Syllable structure and syllable-related processes in German. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Priming effects with phonemically similar words. The encoding-bias hypothesis reconsidered. Memory and Cognition 8, 115–23.Google Scholar
Hollich, G., Jusczyk, P. W. & Luce, P. A. (2002). Lexical neighbourhood effects in 17-month-old word learning. In Scarabela, B., Fish, S. A. & Do, A. (eds), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 314–23. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A. and Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language 33, 630–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kauschke, C. & Siegmüller, J. (2009). Patholinguistische Diagnostik bei Sprachentwicklungsstörungen, 2nd edn.München: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M. M. & Lleó, C. (2003). The acquisition of nuclei. A longitudinal analysis of phonological vowel length in three German-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 30, 527–56.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M. M. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2001). Development of syllable structure in English-speaking children with particular reference to rhymes. Journal of Child Language 28, 393432.Google Scholar
Kiese-Himmel, C. (2005). Aktiver Wortschatztest für 3- bis 5-jährige Kinder – Revision (AWST-R). Göttingen: Beltz.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Davis, J. & Deevy, P. (2007). Phonotactic probability and past tense use by children with specific language impairment and their developing peers. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 21, 747–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, C. C. (1994). On the acquisition of place. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Leiden University, Den Haag.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project. Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A. (1997). Children's productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighbourhood structure. Cognitive Science 21, 283304.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Wulfeck, B. & Ellis Weismer, S. (1999). Productivity of past tense in children with normal language and specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 42, 206219.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. R. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2006). A challenge to current models of past tense inflection. The impact of phonotactics. Cognition 100, 302320.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. R. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). The impact of phonological complexity on past tense inflection in children with grammatical-SLI. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology 9, 191203.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. R., Marinis, T. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Passive verb morphology: The effect of phonotactics on passive comprehension in typically developing and grammatical-SLI-children. Lingua 117, 1434–47.Google Scholar
Matthews, D. E. & Theakston, A. L. (2006). Errors of omission in English-speaking children's production of plurals and the past tense: The effects of frequency, phonology, and competition. Cognitive Science 30, 1027–52.Google Scholar
Mattys, S. L. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition 78, 91121.Google Scholar
McRae, K. & Boisvert, S. (1998). Automatic semantic similarity priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 24, 558–72.Google Scholar
Murphy, V. A., Dockrell, J., Messer, D. & Farr, H. (2008). Morphosyntax in children with word finding difficulties. Journal of Child Language 35, 703720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. & Bjeljac-Babic, R. (2009). A perceptual equivalent of the labial-coronal effect in the first year of life. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 126, 1440–46.Google Scholar
Oetting, J. B. & Hadley, P. A. (2009). Morphosyntax in child language disorders. In Schwartz, R. G. (ed.), Handbook of child language disorders, 341–64. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Oetting, J. B. & Horohov, J. E. (1997). Past-tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 40, 6274.Google Scholar
Penke, M. (2006). Flexion im mentalen Lexikon. Tübingen: Niemeyer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K. & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building. Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48, 2169.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. & Oetting, J. B. (1993). Morphological deficits of children with SLI: Evaluation of number marking and agreement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 1249–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Ruff Noll, K. & Grimm, H. (1997). An extended optional infinitive stage in German-speaking children with specific language impairment. Language Acquisition 6, 255–95.Google Scholar
Richtsmeier, P. T., Gerken, L. A. & Ohala, D. K. (2011). Contributions of phonetic token variability and word-type frequency to phonological representations. Journal of Child Language 38(5), 951–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Learning the past tenses of English verbs. Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing? In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition, 194248. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N. & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, J. Y., Sundara, M. & Demuth, K. (2009). Phonological constraints on children's production of English third person singular -s. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 52, 623–42.Google Scholar
Stokes, S., Wong, A. M.-Y., Fletcher, P. & Leonard, L. B. (2006). Nonword repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of SLI. The case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 49, 219–36.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words. Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 44, 1321–37.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. & Rogers, M. A. (2000). The effect of probabilistic phonotactics on lexical acquisition. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 14, 407425.Google Scholar
Theodore, R. M., Demuth, K. & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2011). Acoustic evidence for positional and complexity effects on children's production of plural -s. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 54, 539–48.Google Scholar
Thiessen, E. D. (2007). The effect of distributional information on children's use of phonemic contrasts. Journal of Memory and Language 56, 1634.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. & Gopnick, M. (1999). The production of inflectional morphology in hereditary specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 20, 51117.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. & Ullman, M. T. (2001). Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 177217.Google Scholar
Verrips, M. & Weissenborn, J. (1992). Routes to verb placement in early German and French. The independence of finiteness and agreement. In Meisel, J. M. (ed.), The acquisition of verb placement, 283331. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S. (2003). The influence of sublexical and lexical representations on the processing of spoken words in English. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 17, 487–99.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. (2000). The phonology of German, 2nd corrected edn.Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zamuner, T. S. (2009). Phonotactic probabilities on the onset of language development. Speech production and word position. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 52, 4960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed