Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-7qhmt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T13:59:13.425Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gesturing with an injured brain: How gesture helps children with early brain injury learn linguistic constructions*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2012

ŞEYDA ÖZÇALIŞKAN*
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
SUSAN C. LEVINE
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
SUSAN GOLDIN-MEADOW
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
*
Address for correspondence: Şeyda Özçalışkan, Georgia State University, Department of Psychology, PO Box 5010, Atlanta, GA 30302-5010. e-mail: seyda@gsu.edu

Abstract

Children with pre/perinatal unilateral brain lesions (PL) show remarkable plasticity for language development. Is this plasticity characterized by the same developmental trajectory that characterizes typically developing (TD) children, with gesture leading the way into speech? We explored this question, comparing eleven children with PL – matched to thirty TD children on expressive vocabulary – in the second year of life. Children with PL showed similarities to TD children for simple but not complex sentence types. Children with PL produced simple sentences across gesture and speech several months before producing them entirely in speech, exhibiting parallel delays in both gesture + speech and speech-alone. However, unlike TD children, children with PL produced complex sentence types first in speech-alone. Overall, the gesture–speech system appears to be a robust feature of language learning for simple – but not complex – sentence constructions, acting as a harbinger of change in language development even when that language is developing in an injured brain.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We thank Kristi Schonwald and Jason Voigt for administrative and technical support, and the project research assistants for their help in data collection and transcription. We also thank the editors, Edith Bavin and Letitia Naigles, and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft. The research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P01 HD40605) to Goldin-Meadow and Levine.

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. P. & Goodwyn, S. W. (1989). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59, 450–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 577660.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camanioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Reilly, J., Wulfeck, B., Druonkers, N., Opie, M., Fenson, J., Kriz, S., Jeffries, R., Miller, L. & Herbst, K. (2001). Differential effects of unilateral lesions on language production in children and adults. Brain and Language 79(2), 223–65.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Thal, D., Trauner, D., Fenson, J., Aram, D., Eisele, J. & Nass, R. (1997). From first words to grammar in children with focal brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology 13, 275343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. & Leonard, L. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Hearing and Language Research 41(5), 185–92.Google Scholar
Booth, J. R., MacWhinney, B., Thulborn, K. R., Sacco, K., Voyvodic, J. T. & Feldman, H. M. (2000). Developmental and lesion effects in brain activation during sentence comprehension and mental rotation. Developmental Neuropsychology 18, 139–69.Google Scholar
Brasky, K., Nikolas, M., Meanwell, C.Levine, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Language development in children with unilateral brain injury: Effects of lesion size. Poster presented at the Symposium in Child Language Disorders, Madison, Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Broaders, S., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136(4), 539–50.Google Scholar
Butcher, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and gesture, 235–58. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, I., Pizzoli, C. & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 41, 1125–35.Google Scholar
Cicone, M., Wapner, W., Foldi, N., Zurif, E. & Gardner, H. (1979). The relation between gesture and language in aphasic communication. Brain and Language 8(3), 324–49.Google Scholar
Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition 106, 1047–58.Google Scholar
Duffy, R., Duffy, J. & Pearson, K. (1975). Pantomime recognition in aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 18, 115–32.Google Scholar
Duffy, R. & Duffy, J. (1981). Three studies of deficits in pantomime expression and pantomime recognition in aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24, 7084.Google Scholar
Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W. & McNeil, N. M. (2001). Divergence of verbal expression and embodied knowledge: Evidence from speech and gesture in children with specific language impairment. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 309331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, H. (2005). Language learning with an injured brain. Language Learning and Development 1(3/4), 265–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, F. & Wishart, J. (1995). The use of pointing and other gestures by children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation 100, 160–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development: Vol.2. Language, thought and culture, 301334. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Geshwind, N. (1975). The apraxias: Neural mechanisms of disorders of learned movement. American Scientist 63, 188–95.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A. & Trueswell, J. T. (2005). Hard words, Language Learning and Development 1(1), 2364.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9(3), 558–65.Google Scholar
Glosser, G., Wiener, M. & Kaplan, E. (1986). Communicative gestures in aphasia. Brain and Language 27, 345–59.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Beilock, S. L. (2010). Action's influence on thought: The case of gesture. Perspectives on Psychological Science 5, 664–74.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Butcher, C. (2003). Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet, 85107. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Cook, S. W. & Mitchell, Z. A. (2009). Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science 20(3), 267–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E. & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science 10(6), 778–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S. & Singer, M. (1999). What the teacher's hands tell the student's mind about math. Journal of Educational Psychology 91, 720–30.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D. & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science 12(6), 516–22.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Singer, M. A. (2003). From children's hands to adults' ears: Gesture's role in the learning process. Developmental Psychology 39(3), 509520.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). ‘I beg your pardon?’: the preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language 13, 455–76.Google Scholar
Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. (1963). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hadley, P. A. (1998). Early verb-related vulnerability among children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 41(6), 1384–97.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. & Smiley, P. (1987). Early word meanings: The case of object names. Cognitive Psychology 19, 6389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E. & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology 45, 337–74.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16, 368–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. & Archibald, Y. (1974). Motor functions of the left hemisphere. Brain 97, 337–50.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics 10, 179202.Google Scholar
Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, P., Banich, M. T. & Duda, E. (1987). Factors affecting cognitive functioning of hemiplegic children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 27, 2735.Google Scholar
Levine, S. C., Kraus, R., Alexander, E., Suriyakham, L. & Huttenlocher, P. (2005). IQ decline following early unilateral brain injury: A longitudinal study. Brain and Cognition 59, 114–23.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. K. & Nelson, K. B. (2001). Epidemiology of perinatal stroke. Current Opinions in Pediatrics 13, 499505.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Feldman, H., Sacco, K. & Valdes-Perez, R. (2000). Online measures of basic language skills in children with early focal brain lesions. Brain and Language 71, 400431.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Miller, R. & Bates, E. (1991). Babble and first words in children with focal injury. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 122.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38, 1136.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children's early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics. In press.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005a). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96(3), B101B113.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005b). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language 32(3), 481505.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture–speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes 28(24), 190217.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Sex differences in language first appear in gesture. Developmental Science 13(5), 752–60.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In Gale Stam, & Mika Ishino, (eds), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture, 163–74. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, L. N. & Kirshner, H. S. (1981). Gestural impairment and gestural ability in aphasia: A Review. Brain and Language 14, 333–48.Google Scholar
Pickett, L. W. (1974). An assessment of gestural and pantomimic deficit in aphasic patients. Acta Symbolica 5, 6986.Google Scholar
Raja Beharelle, A., Dick, A. S., Goulven, J., Solodkin, A., Huttenlocher, P., Levine, S. C. & Small, S. L. (2010). Left hemisphere regions are critical for language in the face of early left focal brain injury. Brain 133, 1707–716.Google Scholar
Rice, M. (1994). Grammatical categories of children with specific language impairment. In Watkins, R. & Rice, M. (eds), Specific language impairments in children, 6989. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Staudt, M., Lidzba, K., Grodd, W., Wildgruber, D., Erb, M. & Krägeloh-Mann, I. (2002). Right-hemispheric organization of language following early left-sided brain lesions: Functional MRI topography. Neuroimage 16(4), 954–67.Google Scholar
Stiles, J., Reilly, J., Paul, B. & Moses, P. (2005). Cognitive development following early brain injury: Evidence for neural adaptation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 136–43.Google Scholar
Thal, D. & Tobias, S. (1992). Communicative gestures in children with delayed onset of oral expressive vocabulary. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 3, 1281–89.Google Scholar
Varney, N. (1978). Linguistic correlates of pantomime recognition in aphasic patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 41, 564–68.Google Scholar
Weckerly, J., Wulfeck, B. & Reilly, J. (2004). The development of morphosyntactic ability in atypical populations: The acquisition of tag questions in children with early focal lesions and children with specific language impairment. Brain & Language 88(2), 190201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed