Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T04:32:36.970Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

PROMPT-TYPE FREQUENCY, AUDITORY PATTERN DISCRIMINATION, AND EFL LEARNERS’ PRODUCTION OF WH-QUESTIONS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 August 2012

Kim McDonough*
Affiliation:
Concordia University
Jindarat De Vleeschauwer
Affiliation:
Chiang Mai University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kim McDonough, Department of Education, Concordia University (LB-583-1), 1455 de Maisonneuve Blvd. W., Montréal, Québec, Canada HG3 1M8; e-mail: kim.mcdonough@concordia.ca.

Abstract

Recently researchers have suggested that syntactic priming may facilitate the production of wh-questions with obligatory auxiliary verbs, particularly when learners are prompted to produce those questions with a wide variety of lexical items (McDonough & Kim, 2009; McDonough & Mackey, 2008). However, learners’ ability to benefit from syntactic priming materials with prompt-type frequency may be mediated by their ability to recognize patterns in aural input. The purpose of this replication study is to confirm the positive impact of prompt-type frequency on learners’ production of wh-questions reported by McDonough and Kim (2009), and to investigate whether its impact is mediated by learners’ auditory pattern-discrimination abilities. Thai learners (n = 43) of English as a foreign language (EFL) carried out three oral tests, two sets of syntactic priming activities, and an auditory pattern-discrimination test over a 4-week period. Half of the learners carried out the syntactic priming activities with low-type-frequency prompts, whereas the other learners received high-type-frequency prompts. The results revealed a significant interaction between Type Frequency × Auditory Pattern Discrimination on the immediate and delayed posttests. The findings are discussed in terms of the potential role of individual cognitive factors in mediating the relationship between syntactic priming and second language (L2) development.

Type
ARTICLES
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baumann, J., & Culligan, B. (1995). Adapted general service list (GSL; West, 1953) [Data file]. Retrieved October 16, 2006, fromhttp://jbauman.com/aboutgsl.html.Google Scholar
Bigelow, M., Delmas, R., Hansen, K., & Tarone, E. (2006). Literacy and the processing of oral recasts in SLA. TESOL Quarterly, 40, 665689.Google Scholar
Bock, K., Dell, G., Chang, F., & Onishi, K. (2007). Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition, 104, 437458.Google Scholar
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., & Cleland, A. (2000). Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B13B25.Google Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., Liversedge, S., Stewart, A., & Urbach, T. (1995). Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 489506.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425455.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2008). Usage-based grammar and second language acquisition. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 216236). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cestnick, L., & Jerger, J. (2000). Auditory temporal processing and lexical/nonlexical reading in developmental dyslexics. Journal of American Academy of Audiology, 11, 501513.Google ScholarPubMed
Chang, F., Dell, G., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Childers, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology, 37, 739748.Google Scholar
Cleland, A., & Pickering, M. (2003). The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 214230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeGraaf, R. (1997). The eXperanto experiment: Effects of explicit instruction on second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 249275.Google Scholar
Díaz, B., Baus, C., Escera, C., Costa, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Brain potentials to native phoneme discrimination reveal the origin of individual differences in learning the sounds of a second language. PNAS, 105, 1608316088. Retrieved March 17, 2009, fromwww.pnas.org_cgi_doi_10.1073_pnas.0805022105.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). Individual differences: Interplay of learner characteristics and learning environment. Language Learning, 59(Suppl. 1), 230248.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. (2002). Frequency effects in language acquisition: A review with implication for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143188.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. (2005). Constructions, chunking and connectionism: The emergence of second language structure. In Doughty, C. & Long, M. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 63103). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Erlam, R. (2005). Language aptitude and its relationship to instructional effectiveness in second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research, 9, 147171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiez, J., Raichle, M., Miezin, F., Petersen, S., Talla, P., & Katz, W. (1995). PET studies of auditory and phonological processes: Effects of stimulus characteristics and task demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 357375.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Hall, M., & Peaker, S. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological sequences on serial recognition and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 130.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A., & Casenhiser, D. (2008). Construction learning and second language acquisition. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 197215). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D. (2008). Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 214238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R., & Kolk, H. (1998). Syntactic persistence in Dutch. Language and Speech, 41, 143184.Google Scholar
Heath, S. M., Hogben, J. H., & Clark, C. D. (1999). Auditory temporal processing in disabled readers with and without oral language delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 637647.Google Scholar
Hummel, K. (2002). Second language acquisition and working memory. In Fabbro, F. (Ed.), Advances in the neurolinguistics of bilingualism: Festschrift for Michel Paradis (pp. 95117). Udine, Italy: Forum.Google Scholar
Hummel, K. (2009). Aptitude, phonological memory, and second language proficiency in nonnovice adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 225249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P. (2004). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 182195.Google Scholar
Kim, Y., & McDonough, K. (2008). Learners’ production of passives during syntactic priming activities. Applied Linguistics, 29, 149154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Children’s first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 168196). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., Philp, J., Egi, T., Fujii, A., & Tatsumi, T. (2002). Individual differences in working memory, noticing of interactional feedback, and L2 development. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 181210). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
McDonough, K. (2005). Identifying the impact of negative feedback and learners’ responses on ESL question development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 79103.Google Scholar
McDonough, K. (2006). Interaction and syntactic priming: English L2 speakers’ production of dative constructions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 179207.Google Scholar
McDonough, K. (2007). Interactional feedback and the emergence of simple past activity verbs in L2 English. In Mackey, A. (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition (pp. 323338). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McDonough, K., & Chaikitmongkol, W. (2010). Collaborative syntactic priming activities and EFL learners’ production of wh-questions. Canadian Modern Language Review, 66, 811835.Google Scholar
McDonough, K., & Kim, Y. (2009). Syntactic priming, type frequency, and EFL learners’ production of wh-questions. Modern Language Journal, 93, 386398.Google Scholar
McDonough, K., & Mackey, A. (2006). Responses to recasts: Repetitions, primed production and linguistic development. Language Learning, 54, 693720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K., & Mackey, A. (2008). Syntactic priming and ESL question development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 3147.Google Scholar
O’Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical, narrative, and grammatical skills in second language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 377402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Freed, B., & Collentine, J. (2007). Phonological memory predicts second language oral fluency gains in adults. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 557582.Google Scholar
Pickering, M., & Branigan, H. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169225.Google Scholar
Ranta, L. (2002). The role of learners’ analytical ability in the communicative classroom. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 159180). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (1997). Individual differences and the fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit adult second language learning. Language Learning, 47, 4599.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2002). Learning conditions, aptitude complexes and SLA: A framework for research and pedagogy. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 113136). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 4673.Google Scholar
Robinson, P., & Ellis, N. (2008). Conclusion: Cognitive linguistics, second language acquisition and L2 instruction—issues for research. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 489545). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Rosen, S., Adlard, A., & van der Lely, H. (2009). Backward and simultaneous masking in children with grammatical specific language impairment: No simple link between auditory and language abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 396411.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. (2007). Explaining errors in children’s questions. Cognition, 104, 106134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, C., Lieven, H., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions. Developmental Science, 6, 557567.Google Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Structural priming as implicit learning: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2, 2749.Google Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R., & Pickering, M. (2007). The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 153171.Google Scholar
Seeff-Gabriel, B. (2003). Phonological processing: A platform for assisting second-language learners with English spelling. Child Language and Teaching Therapy, 19, 291310.Google Scholar
Sheen, Y. (2008). Recasts, language anxiety, modified output, and L2 learning. Language Learning, 58, 835874.Google Scholar
Shin, J., & Christianson, K. (2011). Structural priming and second language learning. Language Learning [Advance online publication]. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00657.x.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2002). Theorising and updating aptitude. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 6994). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2009). Long-term relationships among early first language skills, second language aptitude, second language affect, and later second language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 725755.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1993). The output hypothesis: Just speaking and writing aren’t enough. Canadian Modern Language Review, 50, 158164.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trofimovich, P., Ammar, A., & Gatbonton, E. (2007). How effective are recasts? The role of attention, memory, and analytical ability. In Mackey, A. (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition (pp. 171195). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H., Rosen, S., & Adlard, A. (2004). Grammatical language impairment and the specificity of cognitive domains: Relations between auditory and language abilities. Cognition, 94, 167183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, M., Givens, G., Cranford, J., Holbert, D., & Walker, L. (2006). Auditory pattern recognition and brief tone discrimination of children with reading disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39, 442455.Google Scholar
Walker, M., Shinn, J., Cranford, J., Givens, G., & Holbert, D. (2002). Auditory temporal processing performance of young adults with reading disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 598605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., & Geva, E. (2003). Spelling acquisition of novel English phonemes in Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 325348.Google Scholar
Winkler, I., Kujala, T., Tiitinen, H., Sivonen, P., Alku, P., Lehtokoski, A., et al. . (1999). Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. Psychophysiology, 26, 638642.Google Scholar