Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-94d59 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T22:15:23.525Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's use of phonological information in ambiguity resolution: a view from Mandarin Chinese*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2011

PENG ZHOU*
Affiliation:
Macquarie University
YI (ESTHER) SU
Affiliation:
Macquarie University
STEPHEN CRAIN
Affiliation:
Macquarie University
LIQUN GAO
Affiliation:
Beijing Language and Culture University
LIKAN ZHAN
Affiliation:
Beijing Language and Culture University
*
Address for correspondence: Peng Zhou, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia. tel: +61 2 9850 6735; fax: +61 2 9850 6059; e-mail address: peng.zhou@mq.edu.au

Abstract

How do children develop the mapping between prosody and other levels of linguistic knowledge? This question has received considerable attention in child language research. In the present study two experiments were conducted to investigate four- to five-year-old Mandarin-speaking children's sensitivity to prosody in ambiguity resolution. Experiment 1 used eye-tracking to assess children's use of stress in resolving structural ambiguities. Experiment 2 took advantage of special properties of Mandarin to investigate whether children can use intonational cues to resolve ambiguities involving speech acts. The results of our experiments show that children's use of prosodic information in ambiguity resolution varies depending on the type of ambiguity involved. Children can use prosodic information more effectively to resolve speech act ambiguities than to resolve structural ambiguities. This finding suggests that the mapping between prosody and semantics/pragmatics in young children is better established than the mapping between prosody and syntax.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was partially supported by the Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science Postgraduate Grant to Peng Zhou, and the Chinese National Social Science Grant 09BYY022 to Liqun Gao. We are grateful to two anonymous reviewers and the action editor for their insightful comments and suggestions on an earlier version of the article. We would also like to thank the children and adults who took part in the experiments for their time and patience.

References

REFERENCES

Aoun, J. & Sportiche, D. (1983). On the formal theory of Government. Linguistic Review 2, 211–36.Google Scholar
Arnold, J. E., Brown-Schmidt, S. & Trueswell, J. (2007). Children's use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes 22, 527–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bara, B. G. & Bucciarelli, M. (1998). Language in context: The emergence of pragmatic competence. In Quelhas, A. C. & Pereira, F. (eds), Special issue of Analise Psicologica: Cognition in context, 317–45. Lisbon: Istituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada.Google Scholar
Bernicot, J. & Legros, S. (1987). Direct and indirect directives: What do young children understand? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 43, 346–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt-Kobele, O.-C. & Höhle, B. (2010). What asymmetries within comprehension reveal about asymmetries between comprehension and production: The case of verb inflection in language acquisition. Lingua 120, 1910–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., Colle, L. & Bara, B. G. (2003). How children comprehend speech acts and communicative gestures. Journal of Pragmatics 35, 207241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S. (1991). On the typology of wh-questions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S. (1994). Wh-words as polarity items. Chinese Languages and Linguistics 2, 615–40.Google Scholar
Choi, Y. & Mazuka, R. (2003). Young children's use of prosody in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32, 197217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, J. & Szendröi, K. (2006). Acquisition of focus marking in European Portuguese: Evidence for a unified approach to focus. In Torrens, V. & Escobar, L. (eds), The acquisition of syntax in Romance Languages, 319–29. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. & Thornton, R. (1998). Investigations in Universal Grammar: A guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cruttenden, A. (1985). Intonation comprehension in ten-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 12, 643–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A. & Swinney, D. A. (1987). Prosody and the development of comprehension. Journal of Child Language 14, 145–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garding, E. (1987). Speech act and tonal pattern in Standard Chinese. Phonetica 44, 1329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassmann, S. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words. Journal of Child Language 34, 677–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassmann, S. & Tomasello, M. (2010). Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds' attention to a contextually new referent. Journal of Pragmatics 42, 30983105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gualmini, A., Maciukaite, S. & Crain, S. (2002). Children's insensitivity to contrastive stress in sentences with only. In Arunachalam, S., Kaiser, E. & Williams, A. (eds), Proceedings of the 25th Penn Linguistics Colloquium, 87–110. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Halbert, A., Crain, S., Shankweiler, D. & Woodams, E. (1995). Children's interpretive use of emphatic stress. Poster presented at the 8th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Tucson, AZ.Google Scholar
Hendriks, P. & Koster, C. (2010). Production/comprehension asymmetries in language acquisition. Lingua 120, 1887–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendriks, P. & Spenader, J. (2005/2006). When production precedes comprehension: An optimization approach to the acquisition of pronouns. Language Acquisition 13, 319–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Höhle, B., Berger, F., Müller, A., Schmitz, M. & Weissenborn, J. (2009). Focus particles in children's language: Production and comprehension of auch ‘also’ in German learners from 1 year to 4 years of age. Language Acquisition 16, 3666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, L. R. (1969). A presuppositional approach to only and even. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society 5, 98–107.Google Scholar
Hornby, P. A. & Hass, W. A. (1970). Use of contrastive stress by preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 13, 395–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J. (1982). Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Hüttner, T., Drenhaus, H., van de Vijver, R. & Weissenborn, J. (2004). The acquisition of the German focus particle auch ‘too’: Comprehension does not always precede production. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, on-line supplement www.bu.edu/linguistics/APPLIED/BUCLD/supp.html.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1972). Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jin, S. (1996). An acoustic study of sentence stress in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Johnson, V. E., de Villiers, J. G. & Seymour, H. N. (2005). Agreement without understanding? The case of third person singular /s/. First Language 25, 317–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiss, K. É. (1998). Identificational focus versus informational focus. Language 74, 245–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krifka, M. (1991). A compositional semantics for multiple focus constructions. Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics 10, 127–58.Google Scholar
Li, Y.-H. A. (1992). Indefinite wh in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 1, 125–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, J.-W. (1996). Polarity licensing and wh-phrase quantification in Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst.Google Scholar
Lin, J.-W. (1998). On existential polarity wh-phrases in Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 7, 219–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: A cross-cultural developmental study. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 17, 539–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. (1973). The effects of stress on the understanding of pronominal co-reference in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeder, K. (1980). The emergence of illocutionary skills. Journal of Child Language 7, 1328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinhart, T. (1976). The syntactic domain of anaphora. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Reinhart, T. (2004). The processing cost of reference set computation: Acquisition of stress shift and focus. Language Acquisition 12, 109155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinhart, T. (2006). Interface strategies: Optimal and costly computations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooth, M. (1985). Association with focus. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst.Google Scholar
Rooth, M. (1992). A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics 1, 75–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I. A., Stromswold, K. & Hestvik, A. (2004). How do adults and children process referentially ambiguous pronouns? Journal of Child Language 31, 123–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekerina, I. E. & Trueswell, J. C. (in press). Interactive processing of contrastive expressions by Russian children. First Language.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. & Trueswell, J. C. (2001). Unheeded cues: Prosody and syntactic ambiguity in mother–child communication. Paper presented at the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology 49, 238–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snedeker, J. & Yuan, S. (2008). Effects of prosodic and lexical constraints on parsing in young children (and adults). Journal of Memory and Language 58, 574608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solan, L. (1980). Contrastive stress and children's interpretation of pronouns. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 23, 688–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speer, S. R. & Ito, K. (2009). Prosody in first language acquisition – Acquiring intonation as a tool to organize information in conversation. Language and Linguistics Compass 3, 90–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szendröi, K. (2004). Acquisition evidence for an interface theory of focus. In Van Kampen, J. & Baauw, S. (eds), Proceedings of GALA 2003, 457–68. Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007). Spoken language comprehension: Insights from eye movements. In Gaskell, G. (ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics, 309326. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K. & Sedivy, J. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268, 1632–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trueswell, J. C. (2008). Using eye movements as a developmental measure within psycholinguistics. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernández, E. M. & Clahsen, H. (eds), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing, 7396. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C. & Gleitman, L. R. (2007). Learning to parse and its implications for language acquisition. In Gaskell, G. (ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics, 635–56. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M. & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition 73, 89–134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, B., , S. N. & Yang, Y. F. (2002). The pitch movement of stressed syllable in Chinese sentences. Acta Acustica 27, 234–40 [in Chinese].Google Scholar
Wells, B., Peppé, S. & Goulandris, N. (2004). Intonation development from five to thirteen. Journal of Child Language 31, 749–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Y. (1999). Effects of tone and focus on the formation and alignment of F0 contours. Journal of Phonetics 27, 55–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhong, X. B., Wang, B. & Yang, Y. F. (2001). The perception of prosodic word stress in standard Chinese. Acta Psychologica Sinica 34, 29 [in Chinese].Google Scholar