Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T13:40:55.073Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Survival of Overconfidence in Currency Markets

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 January 2012

Thomas Oberlechner
Affiliation:
Webster University Vienna, Department of Psychology, Berchtoldgasse 1, A-1220 Vienna, Austria. oberlechner@webster.ac.at
Carol Osler
Affiliation:
Brandeis University, International Business School, Mailstop 32, 415 South St., Waltham, MA 02454. cosler@brandeis.edu

Abstract

This paper tests the influential hypothesis that irrational traders will be driven out of financial markets by trading losses. The paper’s main finding is that overconfident currency dealers are not driven out of the market. Dealers with extensive experience are neither more nor less overconfident than their junior colleagues. We set the stage for this investigation by providing evidence that currency dealers display two forms of overconfidence: They underestimate uncertainty, and they overestimate their professional success. This is notable because one might have expected the opposite: currency dealers face strong incentives for accuracy, they have access to comprehensive information, and they have extensive experience.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Michael G. Foster School of Business, University of Washington 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alicke, M. D., and Govorun, O.. “The Better-Than-Average Effect.” In The Self in Social Judgment, Alicke, M. D., Dunning, D. A., and Krueger, J. I., eds. New York: Psychology Press (2005), 85106.Google Scholar
Bank for International Settlements. Triennial Central Bank Survey of Foreign Exchange and Derivatives Market Activity in April 2010—Preliminary Global Results—Turnover. http://www.bis.org/publ/rpfx10.htm (2010).Google Scholar
Barber, B. M., and Odean, T.. “Trading Is Hazardous to Your Wealth: The Common Stock Investment Performance of Individual Investors.” Journal of Finance, 55 (2000), 773806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barber, B. M., and Odean, T.. “Boys Will Be Boys: Gender, Overconfidence, and Common Stock Investment.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116 (2001), 261292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barberis, N. C., and Thaler, R. H.. “A Survey of Behavioral Finance.” In Handbook of the Economics of Finance, Constantinides, G. M., Harris, M., and Stulz, R., eds. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science (2003), 10511121.Google Scholar
Bénabou, R., and Tirole, J.. “Self-Confidence and Personal Motivation.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117 (2002), 871915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhandari, G., and Deaves, R.. “The Demographics of Overconfidence.” Journal of Behavioral Finance, 7 (2006), 511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biais, B.; Hilton, D.; Mazurier, K.; and Pouget, S.. “Judgmental Overconfidence, Self-Monitoring, and Trading Performance in an Experimental Asset Market.” Review of Economic Studies, 72 (2005), 287312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borman, W. C., and Motowidlo, S. J.. “Expanding the Criterion Domain to Include Elements of Contextual Performance.” In Personnel Selection in Organizations, Schmidt, N. and Borman, W. C., eds. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass (1993), 7198.Google Scholar
Bradley, J. V. “Overconfidence in Ignorant Experts.” Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 17 (1981), 8284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandstätter, H. “Die Beurteilung von Mitarbeitern.” In Handbuch für Psychologie, vol. 9, Betriebspsychologie, Mayer, A. and Herwing, B., eds. Hogrefe: Verlag für Psychologie (1970), 668734.Google Scholar
Burnside, C.; Han, B.; Hirshleifer, D.; and Wang, T. Y.. “Investor Overconfidence and the Forward Premium Puzzle.” Review of Economic Studies, 78 (2011), 523558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C., and Lovallo, D.. “Overconfidence and Excess Entry: An Experimental Approach.” American Economic Review, 89 (1999), 306318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, G.; Kim, K. A.; Nofsinger, J. R.; and Rui, O. M.. “Trading Performance, Disposition Effect, Overconfidence, Representativeness Bias, and Experience of Emerging Market Investors.” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20 (2007), 425451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, Y.-W., and Chinn, M. D.. “Currency Traders and Exchange-Rate Dynamics: A Survey of the U.S. Market.” Journal of International Money and Finance, 20 (2001), 439471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniel, K. D.; Hirshleifer, D.; and Subrahmanyam, A.. “Investor Psychology and Security Market Under- and Overreactions.” Journal of Finance, 53 (1998), 18391885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniel, K.; Hirshleifer, D.; and Teoh, S. H.. “Investor Psychology in Capital Markets: Evidence and Policy Implications.” Journal of Monetary Economics, 49 (2002), 139209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deaves, R.; Lüders, E.; and Luo, G. Y.. “An Experimental Test of the Impact of Overconfidence and Gender on Trading Activity.” Review of Finance, 13 (2009), 555575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Long, J. B.; Shleifer, A.; Summers, L. H.; and Waldmann, R. J.. “Positive Feedback Investment Strategies and Destabilizing Rational Speculation.” Journal of Finance, 45 (1990), 379395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Long, J. B.; Shleifer, A.; Summers, L. H.; and Waldmann, R. J.. “The Survival of Noise Traders in Financial Markets.” Journal of Business, 64 (1991), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dittrich, D. A. V.; Guth, W.; and Maciejovsky, B.. “Overconfidence in Investment Decisions: An Experimental Approach.” European Journal of Finance, 11 (2005), 471491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, C.The Forward Discount Anomaly and the Risk Premium: A Survey of Recent Evidence.” Journal of Empirical Finance, 3 (1996), 123192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. D. D., and Lyons, R. K.. “Meese-Rogoff Redux: Micro-Based Exchange-Rate Forecasting.” American Economic Review, 95 (2005), 405414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felson, R. B. “The Effect of Self-Appraisals of Ability on Academic Performance.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47 (1984), 944952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; and Lichtenstein, S.. “Knowing with Certainty: The Appropriateness of Extreme Confidence.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3 (1977), 552564.Google Scholar
Flood, R. P., and Rose, A. K.. “Fixing Exchange Rates: A Virtual Quest for Fundamentals.” Journal of Monetary Economics, 36 (1995), 337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankel, J. A., and Froot, K. A.. “Using Survey Data to Test Standard Propositions Regarding Exchange Rate Expectations.” American Economic Review, 77 (1987), 133153.Google Scholar
Friedman, M.Essays in Positive Economics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (1953).Google Scholar
Gervais, S., and Odean, T.. “Learning to Be Overconfident.” Review of Financial Studies, 14 (2001), 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glaser, M.; Langer, T.; and Weber, M.. “Overconfidence of Professionals and Laymen: Individual Differences Within and Between Tasks? ” Available athttp://ssrn.com/abstract=712583 (2005).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glaser, M., and Weber, M.. “Overconfidence and Trading Volume.” Geneva Risk and Insurance Review, 32 (2007), 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glaser, M., and Weber, M.. “Which Past Returns Affect Trading Volume?Journal of Financial Markets, 12 (2009), 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, S. H. “Foreign Exchange Rate Forecasting Techniques: Implications for Business and Policy.” Journal of Finance, 34 (1979), 415427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gort, C.; Wang, M.; and Siegrist, M.. “Are Pension Fund Managers Overconfident?Journal of Behavioral Finance, 9 (2008), 163170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. “The Totalitarian Ego: Fabrication and Revision of Personal History.” American Psychologist, 35 (1980), 603618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirshleifer, D.Investor Psychology and Asset Pricing.” Journal of Finance, 56 (2001), 15331597.Google Scholar
Hirshleifer, D., and Luo, G. Y.. “On the Survival of Overconfident Traders in a Competitive Securities Market.” Journal of Financial Markets, 4 (2001), 7384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffrage, U. “Overconfidence.” In Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory, Pohl, R. F., ed. New York: Psychology Press (2004), 235254.Google Scholar
Holt, C. A., and Laury, S. K.. “Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects: New Data Without Order Effects.” American Economic Review, 95 (2005), 902904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keren, G.Facing Uncertainty in the Game of Bridge: A Calibration Study.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39 (1987), 98114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, M.; Osler, C. L.; and Rime, D.. “Foreign Exchange Market Structure, Players, and Evolution.” In Handbook of Exchange Rates, Sarno, L., James, J., and Marsh, I., eds. New York and London: Wiley and Sons (2012).Google Scholar
Kyle, A. S., and Wang, F. A.. “Speculation Duopoly with Agreement to Disagree: Can Overconfidence Survive the Market Test?Journal of Finance, 52 (1997), 20732090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lichtenstein, S.; Fischhoff, B.; and Phillips, L. D.. “The Calibration of Probabilities: The State of the Art.” In Decision Making and Change in Human Affairs, Jungermann, H. and de Zeeuw, G., eds. Amsterdam: D. Reidel (1977).Google Scholar
List, J. A. “Does Market Experience Eliminate Market Anomalies?Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118 (2003), 4171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locklear, T. S.; Granger, B. B.; and Veres, J. G.. “Evaluation of a Behaviorally-Based Appraisal System.” Journal of Managerial Issues, 1 (1989), 6675.Google Scholar
MacDonald, R.Expectations Formation and Risk in Three Financial Markets: Surveying What the Surveys Say.” Journal of Economic Surveys, 14 (2000), 69100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malmendier, U., and Tate, G.. “Does Overconfidence Affect Corporate Investment? CEO Overconfidence Measures Revisited.” European Financial Management, 11 (2005), 649659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menkhoff, L.Short-Term Horizons in Foreign Exchange? Survey Evidence from Dealers and Fund Managers.” Kyklos, 54 (2001), 2748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menkhoff, L., and Taylor, M. P.. “The Obstinate Passion of Foreign Exchange Professionals: Technical Analysis.” Journal of Economic Literature, 45 (2007), 936972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, A. H., and Winkler, R. L.. “Probability Forecasting in Meteorology.” Journal of the American Statistical Association, 79 (1984), 489500.Google Scholar
Nicolson, A., ed. Société Générale Dealers Directory. London: Société Générale (2002).Google Scholar
Nofsinger, J. R. The Psychology of Investing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall (2007).Google Scholar
Oberlechner, T.The Psychology of the Foreign Exchange Market. Chichester: John Wiley (2004).Google ScholarPubMed
Oberlechner, T., and Nimgade, A.. “Work Stress and Performance among Financial Traders.” Stress and Health, 21 (2005), 285293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberlechner, T., and Osler, C.. “Overconfidence in Currency Markets.” Working Paper, Brandeis University (2009).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odean, T.Volume, Volatility, Price, and Profit When All Traders Are Above Average.” Journal of Finance, 53 (1998), 18871934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oskamp, S.Overconfidence in Case-Study Judgments.” Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29 (1965), 261265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osler, C. L. “Foreign Exchange Microstructure: A Survey.” In Encyclopedia of Complexity and System Science, Meyers, R. A., ed. New York: Springer (2009).Google Scholar
Pulford, B. D., and Colman, A. M.. “Overconfidence: Feedback and Item Difficulty Effects.” Personality and Individual Differences, 23 (1997), 125133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Régner, I.; Hilton, D.; Cabantous, L.; and Vautier, S.. “Judgmental Overconfidence: One Positive Illusion or Many? ” Working Paper, University of Toulouse (2004).Google Scholar
Roll, R.The Hubris Hypothesis of Corporate Takeovers.” Journal of Business, 59 (1986), 197216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shleifer, A.Inefficient Markets: An Introduction to Behavioral Finance. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2000).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sieber, J. E. “Effects of Decision Importance on Ability to Generate Warranted Subjective Uncertainty.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30 (1974), 688694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soll, J. B., and Klayman, J.. “Overconfidence in Interval Estimates.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30 (2004), 299314.Google ScholarPubMed
Stael von Holstein, C.-A. S.Probabilistic Forecasting: An Experiment Related to the Stock Market.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 8 (1972), 139158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E., and Brown, J. D.. “Illusion and Well-Being: A Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health.” Psychological Bulletin, 103 (1988), 193210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, R.Social Evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings (1985).Google Scholar
van de Venter, G., and Michayluk, D.. “An Insight into Overconfidence in the Forecasting Abilities of Financial Advisors.” Australian Journal of Management, 32 (2008), 545557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, F. A. “Overconfidence, Investor Sentiment, and Evolution.” Journal of Financial Intermediation, 10 (2001), 138170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar