Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T11:59:54.432Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Affective functioning and social cognition in Noonan syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 July 2011

E. Wingbermühle*
Affiliation:
Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Venray, The Netherlands Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands
J. I. M. Egger
Affiliation:
Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Venray, The Netherlands Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands
W. M. A. Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Venray, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Erasmus University Medical Centre, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
I. van der Burgt
Affiliation:
Department of Human Genetics, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
R. P. C. Kessels
Affiliation:
Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Venray, The Netherlands Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department of Medical Psychology, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
*
*Address for correspondence: E. Wingbermühle, M.Sc., Centre of Excellence for Neuropsychiatry, Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Stationsweg 46, 5803 AC Venray, The Netherlands. (Email: pwingbermuhle@vvgi.nl)

Abstract

Background

Noonan syndrome (NS) is a common genetic disorder, characterized by short stature, facial dysmorphia, congenital heart defects and a mildly lowered IQ. Impairments in psychosocial functioning have often been suggested, without, however, systematic investigation in a clinical group. In this study, different aspects of affective processing, social cognition and behaviour, in addition to personal well-being, were assessed in a large group of patients with NS.

Method

Forty adult patients with NS were compared with 40 healthy controls, matched with respect to age, sex, intelligence and education level. Facial emotion recognition was measured with the Emotion Recognition Task (ERT), alexithymia with both the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) and the Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ), and mentalizing with the Theory of Mind (ToM) test. The Symptom Checklist-90 Revised (SCL-90-R) and the Scale for Interpersonal Behaviour (SIB) were used to record aspects of psychological well-being and social interaction.

Results

Patients showed higher levels of cognitive alexithymia than controls. They also experienced more social distress, but the frequency of engaging in social situations did not differ. Facial emotion recognition was only slightly impaired.

Conclusions

Higher levels of alexithymia and social discomfort are part of the behavioural phenotype of NS. However, patients with NS have relatively intact perception of emotions in others and unimpaired mentalizing. These results provide insight into the underlying mechanisms of social daily life functioning in this patient group.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allanson, JE (2007). Noonan syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C, Seminars in Medical Genetics 145C, 274279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arrindell, WA, Bridges, KR, van der Ende, J, St Lawrence, JS, Gray-Shellberg, L, Harnish, R, Rogers, R, Sanderman, R (2001). Normative studies with the Scale for Interpersonal Behaviour (SIB): II. US students: a cross-cultural comparison with Dutch data. Behaviour Research and Therapy 39, 14611479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arrindell, WA, Ettema, JHM (2003). SCL-90: Handleiding bij een Multidimensionele Psychopathologie-Indicator [SCL-90: Manual of a Multidimensional Psychopathology Indicator]. Swets & Zeitlinger: Lisse, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Bagby, RM, Parker, JDA, Taylor, GJ (1994 a). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale – I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 38, 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bagby, RM, Taylor, GJ, Parker, JDA (1994 b). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale – II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 38, 3340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S, Leslie, AM, Frith, U (2007). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duits, A, Kessels, R (2006). Schatten van het premorbide functioneren [Estimating premorbid functioning]. In Neuropsychologische Diagnostiek [Neuropsychological Assessment] (ed. Hendriks, M., Kessels, R., Gorissen, M. and Schmand, B.), pp. 119129. Boom: Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Heiberg, AN, Heiberg, A (1978). A possible genetic contribution to the alexithymia trait. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 30, 205210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jessimer, M, Markham, R (1997). Alexithymia: a right hemisphere dysfunction specific to recognition of certain facial expressions? Brain and Cognition 34, 246258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsson, H, Näätänen, P, Stenman, H (2008). Cortical activation in alexithymia as a response to emotional stimuli. British Journal of Psychiatry 192, 3238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, DA, Portnoy, S, Hill, P, Gillberg, C, Patton, MA (2005). Psychological profile of children with Noonan syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 47, 3538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattila, AK, Poutanen, O, Koivisto, AM, Salokangas, RKR, Joukamaa, M (2007). Alexithymia and life satisfaction in primary healthcare patients. Psychosomatics 48, 523529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montagne, B, Kessels, RPC, de Haan, EHF, Perret, DI (2007). The Emotion Recognition Task: a paradigm to study the perception of facial emotional expressions at different intensities. Perceptual and Motor Skills 104, 589598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montagne, B, Schutters, S, Westenberg, HGM, van Honk, J, Kessels, RPC, de Haan, EHF (2006). Reduced sensitivity in the recognition of anger and disgust in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 11, 389401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, J, Bühner, M, Ellgring, H (2004). The assessment of alexithymia: psychometric properties and validity of the Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences 37, 373391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, HE (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. NFER-Nelson: Windsor.Google Scholar
Noonan, JA, Ehmke, DA (1963). Associated non-cardiac malformations in children with congenital heart disease. Journal of Pediatrics 63, 468470.Google Scholar
Parker, JD, Taylor, GJ, Bagby, RM (1993). Alexithymia and the recognition of facial expressions of emotion. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 59, 197202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierpont, EI, Pierpont, ME, Mendelsohn, NJ, Roberts, AE, Tworog-Dube, E, Rauen, KA, Seidenberg, MS (2010). Effects of germline mutations in the Ras/MAPK signaling pathway on adaptive behavior: cardiofaciocutaneous syndrome and Noonan syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 152A, 591600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarimski, K (2000). Developmental and behavioural phenotype in Noonan syndrome. Genetic Counseling 11, 383390.Google ScholarPubMed
Swart, M, Kortekaas, R, Aleman, A (2009). Dealing with feelings: characterization of trait alexithymia on emotion regulation strategies and cognitive-emotional processing. PloS One 4, e5751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steerneman, P, Meesters, C, Muris, P (2003). TOM-test. Garant: Apeldoorn, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Valera, EM, Berenbaum, H (2001). A twin study of alexithymia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 70, 239246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Burgt, I (2007). Noonan syndrome. Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases 2, 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, WMA, Hendrikx, JLM, Doorakkers, MC, Egger, JIM, van der Burgt, I, Tuinier, S (2004). Alexithymia in Noonan syndrome. Genetic Counseling 15, 4752.Google ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, WMA, Wingbermühle, E, Egger, J, van der Burgt, I, Tuinier, S (2008). Noonan syndrome: psychological and psychiatric aspects. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 146A, 191196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vorst, HCM, Bermond, B (2001). Validity and reliability of the Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences 30, 413434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III and Wechsler Memory Scale III Technical Manual. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonia, TX.Google Scholar
Wingbermühle, E, Egger, J, van der Burgt, I, Verhoeven, W (2009). Neuropsychological and behavioral aspects of Noonan syndrome. Hormone Research 72 (Suppl. 2), 1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witt, DR, Hoyme, HE, Zonana, J, Manchester, DK, Fryns, JP, Stevenson, JG, Curry, CJ, Hall, JG (1987). Lymphedema in Noonan syndrome: clues to pathogenesis and prenatal diagnosis and review of the literature. American Journal of Medical Genetics 27, 841856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, A, Massarano, A, Super, M, Harrington, R (1995). Behavioural aspects and psychiatric findings in Noonan's syndrome. Archives of Disease in Childhood 72, 153155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed