Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T05:44:04.140Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A case-marking cue for filler–gap dependencies in children's relative clauses in Japanese*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 February 2011

TAKAAKI SUZUKI*
Affiliation:
Kyoto Sangyo University
*
Address for correspondence: Takaaki Suzuki, Department of Foreign Languages, Kyoto Sangyo University, Motoyama-Kamigamo, Kita-ku, Kyoto, Japan603-8555. e-mail: takaaki@cc.kyoto-su.ac.jp

Abstract

Object relative clauses have traditionally been thought to be more difficult than subject relative clauses in child English. However, recent studies as well as Japanese data show contradictory results. This study disclosed preschool children's superior performance on object relative clauses in Japanese; however, this dominance disappeared for the children who could use both the nominative and accusative case markers as cues for the comprehension of single-argument sentences. Assuming a filler–gap dependency for the relative clause formation, we suggest that there is no difference in the difficulty between subject and object relative clauses in the grammar of Japanese-speaking children.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The experiments were carried out as a seminar project by the author and the following students: Mari Saito, Masami Sato, Hiroaki Tanaka, Takako Uchida and Yukari Yamada. A portion of this project was presented at the 10th Annual International Conference of the Japanese Society for Language Sciences (JSLS 2008) and will appear in the proceedings. This research was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) 19520373 from The Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Japan, and Kyoto Sangyo University Research Grants.

References

REFERENCES

Bever, T. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structure. In Hayers, J. (ed.), Cognition and development of language, 279362. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Brandt, S., Kidd, E., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2009). The discourse basis of relativization: An investigation of young German and English-speaking children's comprehension of relative clauses. Cognitive Linguistics 20, 539–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J., Tager-Flusberg, H., Hakuta, K. & Cohen, M. (1979). Children's comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 8, 499528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In Coltheart, M. (ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, 559–86. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Ambridge, B. & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology 40, 177–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68, 176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakuta, K. (1981). Grammatical description versus configurational arrangement in language acquisition: The case of relative clauses in Japanese. Cognition 9, 197236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harada, S., Uyeno, T., Hayahsibe, H. & Yamada, H. (1976). On the development of perceptual strategies in children: A case study on the Japanese child's comprehension of the relative clause constructions. Annual Bulletin 10, 199224. Research Institute of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, University of Tokyo.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1999). Processing complexity and filler–gap dependencies across grammars. Language 75, 244–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayashibe, H. (1975). Word order and particles: A developmental study in Japanese. Descriptive and Applied Linguistics 8, 118.Google Scholar
Holmes, V. M. (1973). Order of main and subordinate clauses in sentence perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 417–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwatate, S. (1980). The word-order and case strategies in Japanese children. The Japanese Journal of Psychology 51, 233–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, J. & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30, 580602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mak, W., Vonk, W. & Schriefers, H. (2006). Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language 54, 466–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyamoto, E. & Nakamura, M. (2003). Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. In Garding, G. & Tsujimura, M. (eds), Proceedings of the 22nd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 342–55. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Montag, J. & MacDonald, M. (2009). Word order doesn't matter: Relative clause production in English and Japanese. In Taatgen, N. A. & van Rijn, H. (eds), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2594–99.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2003). Processing of multiple filler–gap dependencies in Japanese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii at Manoa.Google Scholar
O'Grady, W. (1997). Syntactic development. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Grady, W., Lee, M. & Choo, M. (2003). A subject–object asymmetry in the acquisition of relative clauses in Korean as a second language. Studies in Second Language 25, 433–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otsu, Y. (1994) Early acquisition of scrambling in Japanese. In Hoekstra, T. & Schwartz, B. (eds), Acquisition studies in generative grammar, 253–64. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozeki, H. & Shirai, Y. (2007). The consequences of variation in the acquisition of relative clauses: An analysis of longitudinal production data from five Japanese children. In Matsumoto, Y., Oshima, D., Robinson, O. & Sells, P. (eds), Diversity in language: Perspectives and implications, 243–70. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Sano, T. (2004). The acquisition of Japanese topicalization and the role of discourse context. Poster presented at the 28th Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Suzuki, T. (2004). Learning Japanese case: Overextensions and the effects of feedback. In Ettlinger, M., Fleisher, N. & Park-Doob, M. (eds), Proceedings of the Thirtieth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (BLS 30): Conceptual structure and cognition in grammatical theory, 517–28. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Suzuki, T. (2007). Tan-itu-koobun no rikai kara saguru yooji no kakujosi rikai [The development of Japanese case-markers observed through children's comprehension of single-argument sentences]. Gengo Kenkyu 132, 5576.Google Scholar
Tavakolian, S. (1978). The conjoined-clause analysis of relative clauses and other structures. In Goodluck, H. & Solan, L. (eds), Papers in the structure and development of child language, 3783. Amherst, MA: UMass Graduate Linguistic Student Association.Google Scholar