Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T16:24:22.823Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

When cues collide: children's sensitivity to letter- and meaning-patterns in spelling words in English*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2010

S. H. DEACON*
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
D. LEBLANC
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
C. SABOURIN
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
*
Address for correspondence: Hélène Deacon, Psychology Department, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4J1. e-mail: helene.deacon@dal.ca

Abstract

In many learning situations, we need to determine to which cues to attend, particularly in cases when these cues conflict. These conflicts appear often in English orthography. In two experiments, we asked children to spell two-syllable words that varied on two dimensions: morphological and orthographic structure. In one set of these words, the two sources of information conflicted. Results of Experiment 1 suggest that seven- to nine-year-old children are sensitive to both orthographic and morphological dimensions of words, and that this dual sensitivity sometimes leads to correct spelling and sometimes to incorrect spelling. Results of Experiment 2 suggest that orthographic information dominates young (six-year-old) children's spelling, at least in a case when there is a strong orthographic regularity. Taken together, these experiments suggest that children are sensitive to the multiple dimensions of regularity in English orthography and that this sensitivity can lead to mistakes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We are grateful for the generous support from the Chignecto Central School Board and the creative spellings from the children in Enfield and Elmsdale elementary schools.

References

REFERENCES

Bourassa, D. C. & Treiman, R. (2008). Morphological constancy in spelling: A comparison of children with dyslexia and typically developing children. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice 14(3), 155–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassar, M. & Treiman, R. (1997). The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children's knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology 89, 631–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cole, R. A. & Jakimik, J. (1980). A model for speech perception. In Cole, R. (ed.), Perception and production of fluent speech, 136–63. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H. (2008). The metric matters: Determining the extent of children's knowledge of morphological spelling regularities. Developmental Science 11, 396406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, S. H. & Bryant, P. E. (2005). What young children do and do not know about the spelling of inflections and derivations. Developmental Science 8, 583–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H. & Bryant, P. E. (2006). Getting to the root: Young writers' sensitivity to the role of root morphemes in the spelling of inflected and derived words. Journal of Child Language 33, 401417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Conrad, N. & Pacton, S. (2008). A statistical learning perspective on children's learning about graphotactic and morphological regularities in spelling. Canadian Psychology 49, 118–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, M. C., van Engeland, H. & Kemner, C. (2008). Attentional effects of gaze shifts are influenced by emotion and spatial frequency, but not in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 47, 443–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, G. (1970). Relative frequency of English spellings. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. (2005). Learning to read words: Theories, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading 9, 167–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreiro, E. & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. Portsmouth: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K., Coltheart, M. & Marshall, J. (eds), Surface dyslexia, 301330. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hayes, H., Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2006). Children use vowels to help them spell consonants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 94, 2742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, E. (1985). Teaching spelling. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Howell, D. C. (1992). Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd edn.Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, E. K. & Seidl, A. H. (2009). At 11 months, prosody still outranks statistics. Developmental Science 12, 131–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A. & Redanz, N. J. (1993). Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development 64, 675–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemp, N. (2006). Children's spelling of base, inflected, and derived words: Links with morphological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 19, 737–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehtonen, A. & Bryant, P. (2005). Doublet challenge: Form comes before function in children's understanding of their orthography. Developmental Science 8, 211–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology 33, 637–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacton, S. & Deacon, S. H. (2008). The timing and mechanisms of children's use of morphological information in spelling: A review of evidence from French and English. Cognitive Development 23, 339–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S., Fayol, M. & Perruchet, P. (2005). Children's implicit learning of graphotactic and morphological regularities. Child Development 76, 324–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M. & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130, 401426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollo, T. C., Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2008). Three perspectives on spelling development. In Grigorenko, E. J. & Naples, A. (eds), Single-word reading: Cognitive, behavioral, and biological perspectives, 175190. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G. W., Schrijnemakers, J. M. C. & Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with ‘the language-as-fixed-effect fallacy’: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Memory and Language 41, 416–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, C. (1986). Children's creative spelling. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M. & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology 94, 143–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solso, R. L. (1979). Bigram and trigram frequencies and versatilities in the English language. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation 11(5), 475–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solso, R. L. & King, J. F. (1976). Frequency and versatility of letters in the English language. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation 8(3), 283–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treiman, R. & Cassar, M. (1996). Effects of morphology on children's spelling of final consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 63, 141–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R., Cassar, M. & Zukowski, A. (1994). What types of linguistic information do children use in spelling? The case of flaps. Child Development 65, 1318–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, G. S., Bruck, M. & Malus-Abramovitz, M. (1988). The role of linguistic and visual information in spelling: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 45, 400421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeno, S. (ed.) (1995). The educator's word frequency guide. Brewster, NJ: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.Google Scholar