Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T03:27:27.568Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do newly formed word representations encode non-criterial information?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2010

SUZANNE CURTIN*
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
*
Address for correspondence: Suzanne Curtin, Department of Psychology, 2500 University Drive NW, University of Calgary, Calgary, ABCanada, T2N 1N4. tel: 403-220-7670; fax: 403-282-8249; e-mail: scurtin@ucalgary.ca

Abstract

Lexical stress is useful for a number of language learning tasks. In particular, it helps infants segment the speech stream and identify phonetic contrasts. Recent work has demonstrated that infants aged 1 ; 0 can learn two novel words differing only in their stress pattern. In the current study, we ask whether infants aged 1 ; 0 store stress information in their representations of words even when it not required for the task. To this end, we taught infants novel, three-syllable word–object pairings. At test, we manipulated the word by presenting infants with forms that shared the stress pattern of the familiar words but differed in the segments, and forms that shared the segments of the familiar word but differed in the stress pattern. Our findings reveal that infants' representations of new words include word-level stress information and do not simply contain the information critical for distinguishing between different forms.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Thanks to Danielle Droucker, Melanie Khu, and Natasha Nickel for their assistance with this research. I especially thank Susan Graham and Janet Werker for their extremely helpful discussions and comments on this research. I would also like to thank Edith Bavin, the action editor, and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive and insightful comments. I also thank the families who participated in this research. This research was also supported by a SSHRC post-doctoral grant (#756-2001-43), a URGC grant through the University of Calgary, and a SSHRC operating grant (#410-2009-0385).

References

REFERENCES

Bailey, T. M. & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development 17, 1265–82.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Reilly, J. & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21(1), 85–124.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. B., Atkinson, D. J. & Chaput, H. H. (2004). Habit X: A new program for obtaining and organizing data in infant perception and cognition studies (Version 1.0). Austin: University of Texas.Google Scholar
Curtin, S. (2009). Twelve-month-olds learn novel word–object pairings differing only in stress pattern. Journal of Child Language 36, 1157–65.Google Scholar
Curtin, S. (2010). Young infants encode lexical stress in newly encountered words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 105, 376–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtin, S., Mintz, T. H. & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). Stress changes the representational landscape: Evidence from word segmentation. Cognition 96, 233–62.Google Scholar
Echols, C. H., Crowhurst, M. J. & Childers, J. B. (1997). The perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults. Journal of Memory and Language 36, 202–25.Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T. & Waxman, S. R. (in press). What paradox? Referential cues allow for infant use of phonetic detail in word learning. Child Development.Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T. & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners' ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech 46(2), 245–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallé, P. A. & de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1996). The format of representation of recognized words in infants' early receptive lexicon. Infant Behavior and Development 19, 463–81.Google Scholar
Hollich, G., Jusczyk, P. W. & Luce, P. A. (2002). Lexical neighborhood effects in 17-month-old word learning. In Skarabela, B., Fish, S. & Do, A. H.-J. (eds), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1), 314–23. Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, E. K. (2005). English-learning infants' representations of word-forms with iambic stress. Infancy 7, 95–105.Google Scholar
Johnson, E. K. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics. Journal of Memory and Language 44(4), 548–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A. & Redanz, N. J. (1993). Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development 64, 675–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M. & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology 39, 159–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W. & Thompson, E. J. (1978). Perception of a phonetic contrast in multisyllabic utterances by two month-old infants. Perception & Psychophysics 23, 105–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, R. S. (2008). The level of detail in infants' word learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17(3), 229–32.Google Scholar
Pater, J., Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (2004). The lexical acquisition of phonological contrasts. Language 80(3), 361–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature 388, 381–82.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. (2009). Developmental differences in the effects of phonological, lexical, and semantic variables on word learning by infants. Journal of Child Language 36, 291321.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science 13, 480–84.Google Scholar
Thiessen, E. D. (2007). The effect of distributional information on children's use of phonemic contrasts. Journal of Memory and Language 56, 1634.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Nakai, S., Depaolis, R. A. & Hallé, P. (2004). The role of accentual pattern in early lexical representation. Journal of Memory and Language 50, 336–53.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V. L., Casasola, M. & Stager, C. L. (1998). Acquisition of word–object associations by 14-month old infants. Developmental Psychology 34, 1289–309.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development 1(2), 197234.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M. & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants' ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy 3(1), 130.Google Scholar
Yoshida, K. A., Fennell, C. T., Swingley, D. & Werker, J. F. (2009). Fourteen month-old infants learn similar sounding words. Developmental Science 12(3), 112–18.Google Scholar