Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T20:39:09.319Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How specific is the connection between morphological awareness and spelling? A study of French children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 June 2011

S. CASALIS*
Affiliation:
University Charles de Gaulle-Lille Nord deFrance
S. H. DEACON
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
S. PACTON
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes and CNRS FRE 3292
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Severine Casalis, URECA, Universite de Charles de Gaulle Lille 3, BP 60 149, 59653 Villeneuve d'Ascq, France. E-mail: severine.casalis@univ-lille3.fr

Abstract

This study examined the relationship between morphological awareness and spelling. We show that French children in Grades 3 and 4 appear to use morphological information in spelling; spelling of sounds for which there are several alternatives was more accurate in derived than in nonderived words. The link between morphological awareness and spelling seems to be general, given that morphological awareness correlated with multiple spelling scores, including those that did not involve morphology. Further, the relationship between spelling and morphological awareness seems to be affected by both the developmental level of the child and the phonological structure of the items in the morphological awareness task. We discuss the implications of this research for clarifying the relationship between morphological awareness and spelling.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alegria, J., & Mousty, P. (1996). The development of spelling procedures in French-speaking, normal and reading-disabled children: Effects of frequency and lexicality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 312338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anglin, M. J. (1993). Knowing versus learning words. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10, Serial No. 238), 176186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourassa, D., & Treiman, R. (2008). Morphological constancy in spelling: A comparison of children with dyslexia and typically developing children. Dyslexia, 14, 155169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, P., Nunes, T., & Bindman, M. (2000). The relations between children's linguistic awareness and spelling: The case of the apostrophe. Reading and Writing, 12, 253276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 114138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chevrie-Muller, C., Simon, A.-M., & Fournier, S. (1997). Batterie Langage oral. Langage écrit—Mémoire—Attention (L2MA). Paris: Editions du CPA.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H. (2008). The metric matters: Determining the extent of children's knowledge of morphological spelling regularities. Developmental Science, 11, 396406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2006). This turnip's not for turning: Children's morphological awareness and their use of root morphemes in spelling. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 567575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., & Casselman-Bell, M. (2009). How robust is the contribution of morphological awareness to spelling? Reading Psychology, 30, 301318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A., & Liberman, I. (1995). The role of phonology and morphology in morphological awareness. In Feldman, L. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kemp, N. (2006). Children's spelling of base, inflected, and derived words: Links with morphological awareness. Reading and Writing, 19, 737765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary-school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 156166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mousty, P., & Leybaert, J. (1999). Evaluation des habitudes en lecture et orthographe au moyen de BELEC: Données longitudinales auprès d'enfants francophones testés en 2° et 4° primaire. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée, 49, 325342.Google Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997a). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997b). Learning to spell regular and irregular verbs. Reading and Writing, 9, 427449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S., & Deacon, S. H. (2008). The timing and mechanisms of children's use of morphological information in spelling: A review of evidence from French and English. Cognitive Development, 23, 339359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S., Fayol, M., & Perruchet, P. (2005). Children's implicit learning of graphotactic and morphological regularities. Child Development, 76, 324339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, H. (1988). Morphological knowledge and early writing ability. Language and Speech, 31, 337355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sénéchal, M. (2000). Morphological effects in children's spelling of French words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 7686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sénéchal, M., Basque, M., & Leclaire, T. (2006). Morphological knowledge as revealed in children's spelling accuracy and reports of spelling strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 231254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Béchennec, D., & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 167263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R., & Cassar, M. (1996). Effects of morphology on children's spelling of final consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 141170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R., Cassar, M., & Zukowski, A. (1994). What types of linguistic information do children use in spelling? The case of flaps. Child Development, 65, 13181337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Véronis, J. (1988). From sound to spelling in French: Simulation on a computer. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 315334.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Jacobs, A. M., & Stone, G. O. (1996). Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 28, 504515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar