Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T22:36:05.025Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

REANALYSIS IN ADULT HERITAGE LANGUAGE

New Evidence in Support of Attrition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 May 2011

Maria Polinsky*
Affiliation:
Harvard University
*
*Address correspondence to: Maria Polinsky, Department of Linguistics, Harvard University, Boylston Hall Third Floor, Cambridge, MA 02138; e-mail: polinsky@fas.harvard.edu.

Abstract

This study presents and analyzes the comprehension of relative clauses in child and adult speakers of Russian, comparing monolingual controls with Russian heritage speakers (HSs) who are English-dominant. Monolingual and bilingual children demonstrate full adultlike mastery of relative clauses. Adult HSs, however, are significantly different from the monolingual adult controls and from the child HS group. This divergent performance indicates that the adult heritage grammar is not a product of the fossilization of child language. Instead, it suggests that forms existing in the baseline undergo gradual attrition over the life span of a HS. This result is consistent with observations on narrative structure in child and adult HSs (Polinsky, 2008b). Evidence from word order facts suggests that relative clause reanalysis in adult HSs cannot be attributed to transfer from English.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adamec, P. (1966). Porjadok slov v sovremennom russkom jazyke [Word order in modern Russian]. Praha: Rozpravy Českosl. Akad. Ved.Google Scholar
Arnon, I. (2005). Relative clause acquisition in Hebrew: Toward a processing-oriented account. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., & Ha, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 37–48). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Bailyn, J. F. (2004). Generalized inversion. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 22, 1–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, N. J. (1996). Russian learners’ dictionary: 10,000 words in frequency order. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Caplan, D. (2000). Positron emission tomographic studies of syntactic processing. In Grodzinsky, Y., Shapiro, L., & Swinney, D. (Eds.), Language and the brain: Representation and processing (pp. 315–325). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1998). Effects of syntactic structure and prepositional number on patterns of regional blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 541–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1999). PET studies of syntactic processing with auditory sentence presentation. NeuroImage, 9, 343–351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G., & Olivieri, A. (2000). Activation of Broca’s area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Mapping, 9, 65–71.3.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caplan, D., Vijayan, S., Kuperberg, G., West, C., Waters, G., Greve, D., et al. . (2001). Vascular response to syntactic processing: Event-related fMRI study of relative clauses. Human Brain Mapping, 15, 26–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooke, A., Zurif, E. B., DeVita, C., Alsop, D., Koenig, P., Detre, J., et al. . (2002). Neural basis for sentence comprehension: Grammatical and short-term memory components. Human Brain Mapping, 15, 80–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 131–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S., & Lust, B. (1980). Acquisition of relative clauses: Developmental changes in their heads. Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics, 1, 33–45.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 5, 519–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2004). The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: A study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 661–681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., Reznick, J., Dolinski-Nuger, D., & Soboleva, K. (2010). Comprehension and production of movement-derived sentences in Russian speakers with agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 44–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadler, H. (1995). Broca-Aphatiker und das Verstehen von Relativsatzen [Broca aphasia and the understanding of relative clauses]. Grazer Linguistische Monographien, 10, 81–89.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H., Guilfoyle, E., & Harrington, S. (2006). Merge and binding in child relative clauses: The case of Irish. Journal of Linguistics, 42, 629–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodluck, H., & Stojanovič, D. (1996). The structure and acquisition of relative clauses in Serbo-Croatian. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 5, 285–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guasti, M. T., & Cardinaletti, A. (2003). Relative clause formation in Romance child’s production. Probus, 15, 47–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gvozdev, A. N. (1961). Voprosy izučenija detskoj reči [Issues in child language research]. Moscow: Izd. Akad. ped. nauk.Google Scholar
Hamburger, H., & Crain, S. (1982). Relative acquisition. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Language development: Syntax and semantics (pp. 245–274). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hsu, C.-C. B. (2006). Issues in head-final relative clauses in Chinese: Derivation, processing and acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Hsu, C.-C. N., Hermon, G., & Zukowski, A. (2009). Young children’s production of head-final relative clauses: Elicited production data from Chinese children. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 18, 323–360.Google Scholar
Junkal-Gutiérrez, M. (2009, October). Acquiring relatives in L1 Basque. Paper presented at the 34th Boston University Child Language Development Conference, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Just, M., Carpenter, P., & Keller, T. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, E. L. (1976). Remarkable subjects in Malagasy. In Li, C. N. (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 247–301). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and Universal Grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 63–99.Google Scholar
King, J., & Just, M. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, J., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and cause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 376–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, T. H. (1995). Configuring topic and focus in Russian. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kondo-Brown, K. (Ed.). (2006). Heritage language development: Focus on East Asian immigrants. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovtunova, I. (1976). Porjadok slov i aktual´noe členenie predloženija [Word order and information structure of an utterance]. Moscow: Prosvešcˇenie.Google Scholar
Kuno, S. (1973). The structure of Japanese. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kwon, N.-Y. (2008). Processing of syntactic and anaphoric gap-filler dependencies in Korean: Evidence from self-paced reading time, ERP and eye-tracking experiments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Kwon, N.-Y., Lee, Y., Gordon, P., Kluender, R., & Polinsky, M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic factors affecting subject/object asymmetry: An eye-tracking study of pre-nominal relative clauses in Korean. Language, 86, 546–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwon, N.-Y., Polinsky, M., & Kluender, R. (2006). Subject preference in Korean. In Baumer, D., Montero, D., & Scanlon, M. (Eds.), Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 25 (pp. 1–14). Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Levy, R., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2007, March). The syntactic complexity of Russian relative clauses. Paper presented at the City University of New York Sentence Processing Conference, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Pleh, C. (1988). The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition, 29, 95–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K., & Friederici, A. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related brain potentials. Memory and Cognition, 23, 477–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyamoto, E., & Nakamura, M. (2003). Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. In Garding, G. & Tsujimura, M. (Eds.), Proceedings of the West Coast conference on formal linguistics 22 (pp. 342–355). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2002). Incomplete acquisition and attrition of Spanish tense/aspect distinctions in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 39–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morpho-syntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2006). On the bilingual competence of Spanish heritage speakers: Syntax, lexical semantics and processing. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10, 37–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism. Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paducˇeva, E. V. (1985). Vyskazyvanie i ego sootnesennost´s dejstvitel’nost´ju [Utterance and its reference]. Moscow: Nauka.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2000). A composite linguistic profile of a heritage speaker of Russian. In Kagan, O. & Rifkin, B. (Eds.), The learning and teaching of Slavic languages and cultures (pp. 437–466). Bloomington, IN: Slavica Publishers.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2006). Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14, 191–262.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2008a). Gender under incomplete acquisition: Heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. The Heritage Language Journal, 6, 40–71. Retrieved September 3, 2009, fromhttp://www.heritagelanguages.org/CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2008b). Heritage language narratives. In Brinton, D., Kagan, O., & Bauckus, S. (Eds.), Heritage language education: A new field emerging (pp. 108–156). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Kagan, O. (2007). Heritage languages: In the ‘wild’ and in the classroom. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, 368–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Kluender, R. (2007). Linguistic typology and theory construction: Common challenges ahead. Linguistic Typology, 11, 273–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2007). Heritage speaker competence differences, language change and input type: Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11, 359–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russian National Corpus. Retrieved August 8, 2009, fromhttp://www.ruscorpora.ru/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Say, S. (2005, September). Antipassives and A-relativization: Between syntax and discourse pragmatics. Paper presented at the 4th Typological School, Tsakhkadzor, Armenia.Google Scholar
Schriefers, H., Friederici, A., & Kühn, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 499–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, F. (2007). Processing presupposed language. Journal of Semantics, 24, 373–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1994). Language contact and change: Spanish in Los Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2008). Meaning in the second language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. (1986). The acquisition and use of relative clauses in Turkic and Indo-European languages. In Slobin, D. & Zimmer, K. (Eds.), Studies in Turkish linguistics (pp. 277–298). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language, 52, 452–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tjung, Y. N. (2006). The formation of relative clauses in Jakarta Indonesian: A subject-object asymmetry. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Traxler, M., Morris, R., & Seely, R. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 69–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007a). The bilingual child: Early development and language contact. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007b). Relative clauses in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Typological challenges and processing motivations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 277–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar