Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T10:05:56.851Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mastering inflectional suffixes: a longitudinal study of beginning writers' spellings*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2010

KATHRYN TURNBULL
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
S. HÉLÈNE DEACON*
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
ELIZABETH KAY-RAINING BIRD
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
*
[*]Address for correspondence: Hélène Deacon, Psychology Department, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4J1. E-mail: helene.deacon@dal.ca

Abstract

This study tracked the order in which ten beginning spellers (M age=5 ; 05; SD=0·21 years) mastered the correct spellings of common inflectional suffixes in English. Spellings from children's journals from kindergarten and grade 1 were coded. An inflectional suffix was judged to be mastered when children spelled it accurately in 90 percent of the contexts in which it was grammatically required, a criterion used to study the order of acquisition of grammatical morphemes in oral language. The results indicated that the order in which children learned to spell inflectional suffixes correctly is similar to the order in which they learn to use them in oral language, before school age. Discrepancies between the order of mastery for inflectional suffixes in written and oral language are discussed in terms of English spelling conventions, which introduce variables into the spelling of inflected words that are not present in oral language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Beers, C. S. & Beers, J. W. (1992). Children's spelling of English inflectional morphology. In Templeton, S. & Bear, D. R. (eds), The development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy, 231–52. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Beers, J. & Henderson, E. (1977). A study of developing orthographic concepts amongst first graders. Research in the Teaching of English 11, 133–48.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryant, P., Nunes, T. & Bindman, M. (2000). The relations between children's linguistic awareness and spelling: The case of the apostrophe. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 12, 253–76.Google Scholar
Bryant, P., Nunes, T. & Snaith, R. (2000). Children learn an untaught rule of spelling. Nature 403, 157.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1996). An exploratory study of morphological errors in children's written stories. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8, 6172.Google Scholar
Cunningham, A. E. & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology 82, 733–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H. & Bryant, P. (2005). What young children do and do not know about the spelling of inflections and derivations. Developmental Science 8, 583–94.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Conrad, N. & Pacton, S. (2008). A statistical learning perspective on children's learning about graphotactic and morphological regularities in spelling. Special issue of Canadian Psychology on literacy development, 49, (2), 118–24.Google Scholar
Derwing, B. L., Smith, M. L. & Wiebe, G. E. (1995). On the role of spelling in morpheme recognition: Experiments with children and adults. In Feldman, L. B. (ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing, 328. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. A. (1973). A cross-sectional study: The development of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2, 267–78.Google Scholar
Kay-Raining Bird, E., Bedrosian, J. L., Rice, R. M. & Szeto, K. (1999). Interactions in written language: Trade-offs between spelling accuracy and lexical or syntactic complexity. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology 23(2), 8190.Google Scholar
Kemp, N. & Bryant, P. (2003). Do beez buzz? Rule-based and frequency-based knowledge in learning to spell plural -s. Child Development 74, 6374.Google Scholar
Munakata, Y. (2001). Graded representations in behavioral dissociations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5, 309315.Google Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. & Bindman, M. (1997a). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology 33, 637–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. & Bindman, M. (1997b). Learning to spell regular and irregular verbs. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 9, 427–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S. & Deacon, S. H. (2008). The timing and mechanisms of children's use of morphological information in spelling: A review of evidence from French and English. Cognitive Development 23, 339–59.Google Scholar
Pollo, T. C., Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2007). Three perspectives on spelling development. In Grigorenko, E. J. & Naples, A. J. (eds), Single-word reading: Cognitive, behavioral, and biological perspectives, 175–89. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Read, C. (1986). Children's creative spelling. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Rubin, H. (1988). Morphological knowledge and early writing ability. Language and Speech 31, 337–55.Google Scholar
Russo, R. (2003). Statistics for the behavioural sciences: An introduction. USA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Scott, C. M. (1988). Spoken and written syntax. In Nippold, M. A. (ed.), Later language development: Ages nine through thirteen, 4995. Boston, MA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Lundquist, E., Dreyer, L. G. & Dickinson, C. C. (1996). Reading and spelling difficulties in high school students: Causes and consequences. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8, 267–94.Google Scholar
Stuart, M., Dixon, M. & Masterson, J. (2004). Use of apostrophes by six to nine year old children. Educational Psychology 24, 251–61.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. & Bourassa, D. C. (2000). The development of spelling skill. Topics in Language Disorders 20(3), 118.Google Scholar