Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T01:31:11.674Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The power of social structure: how we became an intelligent lineage

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2010

Marina Resendes de Sousa António*
Affiliation:
Washington State University, School of Earth & Environmental Sciences, PO Box 642812, PullmanWA99164-2812, USA
Dirk Schulze-Makuch
Affiliation:
Washington State University, School of Earth & Environmental Sciences, PO Box 642812, PullmanWA99164-2812, USA

Abstract

New findings pertinent to the human lineage origin (Ardipithecus ramidus) prompt a new analysis of the extrapolation of the social behavior of our closest relatives, the great apes, into human ‘natural social behavior’. With the new findings it becomes clear that human ancestors had very divergent social arrangements from the ones we observe today in our closest genetic relatives.

The social structure of chimpanzees and gorillas is characterized by male competition. Aggression and the instigation of fear are common place. The morphology of A. ramidus points in the direction of a social system characterized by female-choice instead of male–male competition. This system tends to be characterized by reduced aggression levels, leading to more stable arrangements. It is postulated here that the social stability with accompanying group cohesion propitiated by this setting is favorable to the investment in more complex behaviors, the development of innovative approaches to solve familiar problems, an increase in exploratory behavior, and eventually higher intelligence and the use of sophisticated tools and technology.

The concentration of research efforts into the study of social animals with similar social systems (e.g., New World social monkeys (Callitrichidae), social canids (Canidae) and social rodents (Rodentia)) are likely to provide new insights into the understanding of what factors determined our evolution into an intelligent species capable of advanced technology.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avital, E. & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal Traditions: Behavioral Inheritance in Evolution Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bapteste, E. & Boucher, Y. (2008). Trends Microbiol. 16, 200207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begun, D.R. (2005). Amer. J. Phys. Anthropol. 337, 1163.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. (2002). Minding animals: Awareness, Emotions, and Heart. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bushati, N. & Cohen, S.M. (2007). Annu. Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 23, 175205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterfield, N.J. (2007). Palaeontology 50, 4155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chesner, C.A., Halsor, S.P. & Dolan, M.T. (2008). A bathymetric survey of Lake Toba, Indonesia: further results. American Geophysical Union, Abstract #VIIC-2071.Google Scholar
Deardorff, J., Haisch, B., Maccabee, B. & Puthoff, H.E. (2005). J. Brit. Interplanet. Soc. 58, 4350.Google Scholar
De Waal, F. (2007). Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes. The Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.I.M. & Schultz, S. (2007). Science 317, 13441347.Google Scholar
Emery, N.J. (2006). Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 2343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgan, D.H. & Rice, K. (2010). Int. J. Astrobiol. 9, 7380.Google Scholar
Grippo, A.J., Lamb, D.G., Carter, C.S. & Porges, S.W. (2007). Biolog. Psych. 62, 11621170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosenick, L., Clement, T.S. & Fernald, R.D. (2007). Nature 445, 429432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B. (2009). What is the effect of affect on Bonobo and Chimpanzee problem solving? In Neurobiology of “Umwelt”, pp. 89–102. Springer, Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, A.A. (2009). Futures 41, 554561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, W.J.E., Brown, G.R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M.M. & Laland, K.N. (2008). Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 486493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishida, H., Tuttle, R., Pickford, M., Ogihara, N. & Nakatsukasa, M. (2006). Human Origins and Environmental Backgrounds. Springer Science+Business Media Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, A.J., Douglas, C.M.S., Huchard, E., Isaac, N.J.B. & Cowlishaw, G. (2008). Current Biol. 18, 18331838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Komdeur, J. (2006). Ethology 112, 729747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lineweaver, C. (2007). Invited talk. In Proceedings of the SETI Workshop Sounds of Silence, Tempe, AZ, 5–7 February, 2007.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, C.O. (2009). Science 326, 74e174e8.Google Scholar
Mainguy, J., Côté, S.D., Cardinal, E. & Houle, M. (2008). J. Mammal. 89, 626635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meister, G. & Tuschl, T. (2004). Nature 431, 343349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michaud, M.A.G. (2007). Contact with Alien Civilizations: our Hopes and Fears about Encountering Extraterrestrials. Copernicus Books, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muller, M.N. & Wrangham, R.W. (2009). Sexual Coercion in Primates and Humans. Harvard College.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oppenheimer, C. (2002). Quat. Sci. Rev. 21, 15931609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plavcan, J.M. & Ruff, C.B. (2008). Amer. J. Phys. Anthropol. 136, 6584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pradhan, G.R. & van Schaik, C. (2008). Behaviour 145, 251275.Google Scholar
Raichle, M.E. (2006). Science 314, 12491250.Google Scholar
Reznikova, Z. (2007). Animal Intelligence: From Individual to Social Cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rylands, A.B. & Mittermeier, R.A. (2009). South American Primates: Comparative Perspectives in the Study of Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. Springer.Google Scholar
Sorek, R., Zhu, Y., Creevey, C.J., Francino, M.P., Bork, P. & Rubin, E.M. (2007). Science 318, 14491452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, C.M. & Nielsen, K.M. (2005). Nature Rev. Microbiol. 3, 711721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, S.W., Slocombe, K.E., Thompson, M.E. & Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Current Biol. 17, R355R356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, P. & Brownlee, D. (2000). Rare Earth: Why Complex Life is Uncommon in the Universe. Copernicus Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webb, S. (2002). If the Universe is Teeming with Aliens … Where is Everybody? Fifty Solutions to the Fermi Paradox and the Problem of Extraterrestrial Life. Copernicus Books, New York.Google Scholar
White, T.D., Asfaw, B., Beyene, Y., Haile-Selassie, Y., Lovejoy, C.O., Suwa, G. & WoldeGabriel, G. (2009). Science 326, 7586.Google Scholar