Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T21:22:57.577Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of outliers on the perception of sound change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 November 2010

William Labov
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Maciej Baranowski
Affiliation:
University of Manchester
Aaron Dinkin
Affiliation:
Swarthmore College

Abstract

The role of outliers in vowel distributions is examined through an experiment that registers subjects' assessments of symmetrical and asymmetrical distributions. Subjects first scaled their subjective impressions of a series of six resynthesized tokens of the word bad ranging from low front to upper mid front. They were then asked to register on this scale their overall impressions of four series of five phrases including bad: low symmetrical, low with a very low outlier, high symmetrical, high with a very high outlier. Subjects show the capacity to integrate outliers into their overall assessments in a manner consistent with their acoustic properties. The effect of low outliers was significantly greater, reflecting the socially marked status of this form in the Philadelphia community.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baranowski, Maciej. (2007). Phonological variation and change in the dialect of Charleston, South Carolina. Publication of the American Dialect Society 92. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1966). The social stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1994). Principles of linguistic change. Volume I: Internal factors. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (2001). Principles of linguistic change. Volume 2: Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (2010). Principles of linguistic change. Volume 3: Cognitive and cultural factors. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William, Ash, Sharon, & Boberg, Charles. (2006). Atlas of North American English: Phonology and sound change. Berlin: Mouton/de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Labov, William, Yaeger, Malcah, & Steiner, Richard. (1972). A quantitative study of sound change in progress. Philadelphia: U.S. Regional Survey.Google Scholar
National Institute of Standards and Technology (NIST) and International SEMATECH. (2010 [2003]). NIST/SEMATECH e-Handbook of Statistical Methods. Available at: http://www.itl.nist.gov/div898/handbook/.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet. (2002). Word-specific phonetics. In Gussenhoven, C. & Warner, N. (eds.), Laboratory phonology. Vol. 3. Berlin: Mouton/de Gruyter. 101140.Google Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian. (2005). Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in sociolinguistics. In Ammon, U., Dittmar, N., Mattheier, K. J., & Trudgill, P. (eds.), An international handbook of the science of language and society. Vol. 2. Berlin: de Gruyter. 10031013.Google Scholar
StatSoft. Statistics glossary. Available at: http://www.statsoft.com/textbook/glosfra.html.Google Scholar