Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-995ml Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T23:53:33.950Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parents' use of conventional and unconventional labels in conversations with their preschoolers*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2009

ANNETTE M. E. HENDERSON*
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
MARK A. SABBAGH
Affiliation:
Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario
*
Address for correspondence: Annette M. E. Henderson, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New Zealand. e-mail: a.henderson@auckland.ac.nz

Abstract

Parents' use of conventional versus unconventional labels with their two- (n=12), three- (n=12) and four-year-old children (n=12) was assessed as they talked about objects that were either known or unknown to them. For known objects, parents provided typical conventional labels casually during the conversation. For unknown objects, parents were less likely to use typical nouns as labels and marked their labels with additional information suggesting that the labels might be unconventional. Parents marked potentially unconventional labels by providing explicit statements of ignorance and paralinguistic cues of uncertainty. These patterns were strongest when the unknown objects were manufactured as opposed to homemade, possibly because manufactured objects are supposed to have conventional names that parents were unable to provide. Parents' marking of unconventional labels may help children recognize when new word forms should be treated with caution and guide their learning accordingly.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We would like to extend our gratitude to the parents and children who volunteered their time to participate in this research. We would also like to thank the research assistants who helped with transcribing and coding of the data: R. DiGiovanni, L. Hong, M. K. Friedman, C. Lang, S. Lue and N. Strang. A special thank you to M. Hurt for her assistance with graphics. We would like to thank the members of the Early Experience Laboratory and the Infant and Child Development Group at Queen's University for their helpful comments on this research. Lastly, we would like to thank N. Akhtar, K. Munhall, S. Fitneva, E. Kelley and A. Woodward for their comments on a previous version of this manuscript. This research was supported by a graduate scholarship awarded to A. M. E. Henderson from the Natural Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and an operating grant awarded to M. A. Sabbagh from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

References

REFERENCES

Birch, S. A. & Bloom, P. (2002). Preschoolers are sensitive to the speaker's knowledge when learning proper names. Child Development 73, 434–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Buresh, J. S. & Woodward, A. L. (2007). Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition 104, 287314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callanan, M. (1985). How parents label objects for young children: The role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development 56, 508523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M. & Sabbagh, M. A. (2004). Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents' language and children's developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology 40, 746–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds), Handbook of child psychology (Volume 3). Cognitive Development, 787840. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2007). Conventionality and contrast in language and language acquisition. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 115, 1123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Fox-Tree, J. E. (2002). Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition 84, 73–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleave, P. L. & Bird, E. K.-R. (2006). Effects of familiarity on mothers' talk about nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language 33, 661–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clément, F., Koenig, M. & Harris, P. (2004). The ontogenesis of trust. Mind & Language 19, 360–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. Child Development 64, 637–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, S. A., Stock, H. & Henderson, A. M. E. (2006). Nineteen-month-olds' understanding of the conventionality of object labels versus desires. Infancy 9, 341–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, D. G., Burns, T. C. & Pawluski, J. L. (2003). Input and word learning: Caregivers' sensitivity to lexical category distinctions. Journal of Child Language 30, 711–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Henderson, A. M. E. & Graham, S. A. (2005). Two-year-olds' appreciation of the shared nature of novel object labels. Journal of Cognition and Development 6, 381402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, A. M. E., Woodward, A. L., Bonny, J., Smith, J. & Perez Rojas, A. (2009). Do 9-month-olds appreciate the shared nature of new linguistic forms? Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, Colorado.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 74, 1368–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversations in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development 62, 782–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, M. A., Clément, F. & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science 15, 694–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, M. A. & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development 76, 1261–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. F. (1997). Maternal labeling of novel and familiar objects: Implications for children's development of lexical constraints. Journal of Child Language 24, 427–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book-reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology 19, 445–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language 5, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D. & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 76, 763–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A. & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development 72, 10541070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A. & Callanan, M. (1998). Metarepresentation in action: 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds' developing theories of mind in parent–child conversations. Developmental Psychology 34, 491502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A. & Henderson, A. M. E. (2007). How an appreciation of conventionality shapes early word learning. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 115, 2537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., Wdowiak, S. D. & Ottaway, J. M. (2003). Do word learners ignore ignorant speakers? Journal of Child Language 30, 905924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed