Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T08:19:07.578Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Segmental production in Mandarin-learning infants*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2009

LI-MEI CHEN
Affiliation:
National Cheng Kung University, Taiwan, ROC
RAYMOND D. KENT*
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
*
Address for correspondence: Raymond D. Kent, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI, 53705, USA. e-mail: Kent@Waisman.Wisc.edu

Abstract

The early development of vocalic and consonantal production in Mandarin-learning infants was studied at the transition from babbling to producing first words. Spontaneous vocalizations were recorded for 24 infants grouped by age: G1 (0 ; 7 to 1 ; 0) and G2 (1 ; 1 to 1 ; 6). Additionally, the infant-directed speech of 24 caregivers was recorded during natural infant–adult interactions to infer language-specific effects. Data were phonetically transcribed according to broad categories of vowels and consonants. Vocalic development, in comparison with reports for children of other linguistic environments, exhibited two universal patterns: the prominence of [ɛ] and [ə], and the predominance of low and mid vowels over high vowels. Language-specific patterns were also found, e.g. the early appearance and acquisition of low vowels [ɑ]. Vowel production was similar in G1 and G2, and a continuum of developmental changes brought infants' vocalization closer to the adult model. Consonantal development showed two universal patterns: labials and alveolars occurred more frequently than velars; and nasals developed earlier than fricatives, affricates and liquids. We also found two language-specific patterns: alveolars were more prominent than labials and affricates developed early. Universal and language-specific characteristics in G1 continued to be prominent in G2. These data indicate that infants are sensitive to the ambient language at an early age, and this sensitivity influences the nature of their vocalizations.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We are grateful to the infants and their families who participated in this study. Without their cooperation, this study would not have been possible. Their hospitality made the whole recording procedure an enjoyable experience.

References

REFERENCES

Buhr, R. (1980). The emergence of vowels in an infant. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 23, 7379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M. & Price, C. J. (2008). Brain activation for consonants and vowels. Cerebral Cortex 18(7), 1727–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, K. E., Onishi, K. H. & Fisher, C. L. (2003). Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience. Cognition 87, B69B77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H. P. & Irwin, O. C. (1946). Infant speech: Vowel and consonant types. Journal of Speech Disorders 11, 2729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, L.-M. (2005). Development of vocalic production in a Mandarin-learning infant: A longitudinal case study. Providence Forum: Language and Humanities 1(1), 169–92.Google Scholar
Chen, L.-M. & Kent, R. D. (2005). Consonant–vowel co-occurrence patterns in Mandarin-learning infants. Journal of Child Language 32(3), 507534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, C.-C. (1973). A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, C.-M. (1982). Analysis of present-day Mandarin. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 10, 281358.Google Scholar
Cheng, R. L. (1966). Mandarin phonological structure. Journal of Linguistics 2(2), 135262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, B. L. & MacNeilage, P. F. (1990). Acquisition of correct vowel production: A quantitative case study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33, 1627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1993). Ontogeny of language-specific syllabic productions. In de Boysson-Bardies, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., MacNeilage, P. & Morton, J. (eds), Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life, 353–63. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Hallé, P., Sagart, L. & Durand, C. (1989). A crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journal of Child Language 16, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boysson-Bardies, B. & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67, 297319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Vihman, M. M., Roug-Hellichius, L., Durand, C., Landberg, I. & Arao, F. (1992). Material evidence of infant selection from target language: A cross-linguistic phonetic study. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: Models, research, implications, 369–91. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Gerken, L. A. (2004). Nine-month-olds extract structural principles required for natural language. Cognition 93, B89B96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, J. H. (2003). A study of the stages of development and acquisition of Mandarin Chinese by children in Taiwan. Taipei: Crane Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1989). Phonological disability in children, 2nd edn. London: Whurr Publishers Limited.Google Scholar
Irwin, O. C. (1947 a). Infant speech: Consonantal sounds according to place of articulation. Journal of Speech Disorders 12, 397401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, O. C. (1947 b). Infant speech: Consonant sounds according to manner of articulation. Journal of Speech Disorders 12, 402404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, O. C. (1948). Infant speech: Development of vowel sounds. Journal of Speech Disorders 13, 3134.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941/68). Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton [English translation, 1968].Google Scholar
Jeng, H.-H. (1979). The acquisition of Chinese phonology in relation to Jakobson's law of irreversible solidarity. Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences 2, 155–61. Copenhagen: University of Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1992). The biology of phonological development. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: Models, research, implications, 6590. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Bauer, H. R. (1985). Vocalizations of one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 12, 491526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Murray, A. D. (1982). Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 72(2), 353–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kent, R. D. & Vorperian, H. K. (1995). Anatomic development of the craniofacial-oral-laryngeal systems: A review. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology 3, 145–90.Google Scholar
Li, P. J.-K. (1978). Child language acquisition of Mandarin phonology. In Cheng, R. L., Li, Y.-C. & Tang, T.-C. (eds), Proceedings of the Symposium on Chinese Linguistics, 295316. Taipei: Student Book Co.Google Scholar
Lin, Y. H. (1989). Autosegmental treatment of segmental processes in Chinese phonology. PhD dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
Liu, I.-M., Chuang, C.-J. & Wang, S.-C. (1975). Frequency count of 40 000 Chinese words. Taipei: Lucky Books Company.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Luo, C.-C. (1992). Kuo Yü hsüeh [Studies of Mandarin Chinese]. Taipei: Wu-Nan.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F., Davis, B. L. & Matyear, C. L. (1997). Babbling and first words: Phonetic similarities and differences. Speech Communication 22(2–3), 269–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2008). Fourteen-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Development Science 11(1), 5359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matyear, C. L. (1998). An acoustical study of vowels in babbling. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Mowrer, D. E. (1980). Phonological development during the first year of life. In Lass, N. J. (ed.), Speech and language: Advances in basic research and practice, 99–142. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nishitani, N., Schurmann, M., Amunts, K. & Hari, R. (2005). Broca's region: From action to language. Physiology 20, 6069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollock, K. E. (2002). Identification of vowel errors: Methodological issues and preliminary data from the Memphis Vowel Project. In Ball, M. J. & Gibbon, F. E. (eds), Vowel disorders, 83–113. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.Google Scholar
Pye, C., Ingram, D. & List, H. (1987). A comparison of initial consonant acquisition in English and Quiché. In Nelson, K. & van Kleeck, A. (eds), Children's Language, Vol. 6, 175–90. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
So, L. K. H. & Dodd, B. J. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 22, 473–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stål, P., Marklund, S., Thornell, L. E., de Paul, R. & Eriksson, P.-O. (2003). Composition of human intrinsic tongue muscles. Cell Tissues Organs 173(3), 147–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2002). Intervocalic consonants in the speech of typically developing children: Emergence and early use. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16(3), 155–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gamman, C. & Pollock, K. (2008). Vowel development and disorders. In Ball, M. J., Perkins, M. R., Muller, N. & Howard, S. (eds), The handbook of clinical linguistics, 525–48. Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, S. F. & Wong, I. M. (2002). Vowel and diphthong development in Cantonese-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16, 597617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tse, J. K.-P. (1992). Production and perception of syllable final [n] and [ŋ] in Mandarin Chinese: An experimental study. Studies in English Literature & Linguistics. Taipei: Department of English, National Taiwan Normal University.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1992). Early syllables and the construction of phonology. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: Models, research, implications, 393422. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Kay, E., de Boysson-Bardies, B., Durand, C. & Sundberg, U. (1994). External sources of individual differences? A cross-linguistic analysis of the phonetics of mothers' speech to one-year-old children. Developmental Psychology 30, 651–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H. & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling to speech: A reassessment of the continuity issue. Language 61, 397445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, F., Huang, H., Chen, C.-W. & Phillips, P. (1986). Articulation development in Mandarin-speaking preschool children ages 3–6 in Taiwan. Chang Gung Medical Journal 9, 6875.Google Scholar
Waterson, N. (1978). Growth of complexity in phonological development. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. E. (eds), The development of communication, 415–42. Chichester: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Wong, W. W.-Y. & Stokes, S. F. (2001). Cantonese consonantal development: Towards a nonlinear account. Journal of Child Language 28, 195212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yue-Hashimoto, A. O. (1980). Word play in language acquisition: A Mandarin case. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 8, 181204.Google Scholar
Zhu, H. (2002). Phonological development in specific context: Studies of Chinese-speaking children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Zhu, H. & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (Modern Standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language 27, 342.Google Scholar