Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T04:31:02.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

LEARNERS' USES OF TWO TYPES OF WRITTEN FEEDBACK ON A L2 WRITING REVISION TASK

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2007

Rebecca Sachs
Affiliation:
Georgetown University
Charlene Polio
Affiliation:
Michigan State University

Abstract

This study examines the effectiveness of written error corrections versus reformulations of second language learners' writing as two means of improving learners' grammatical accuracy on a three-stage composition-comparison-revision task. Concurrent verbal protocols were employed during the comparison stage in order to study the learners' reported awareness of the more targetlike reformulations. The reactivity of think-alouds as a research tool was also assessed. First, 15 adult learners of English participated in a repeated-measures study with three experimental conditions: error correction, reformulation, and reformulation + think-aloud. Participant reports of awareness in the reformulation + think-aloud condition suggested that noticing of feedback was related to the accuracy of subsequent revisions. A second nonrepeated-measures study was then carried out with 54 participants; a control group was added and the design was modified in an attempt to eliminate the reported tendency of learners to develop and use memorization strategies while processing the written feedback. In both experiments, participants performed significantly better in the error correction condition than in the reformulation condition. The think-alouds, used to examine learners' attentional processes, were found to be reactive in the first study; learners in the reformulation condition produced significantly more accurate revisions than those who were asked to think aloud while processing the reformulations they received. The results suggest that whereas verbal protocols might be able to shed some light on learner-internal processes in relation to written feedback, they should be employed and interpreted with care.We would like to thank Alison Mackey and the anonymous SSLA reviewers for their helpful suggestions on this article.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586595.Google Scholar
Bowles, M.A. & Leow, R.P. (2005). Reactivity and type of verbal report in SLA research methodology: Expanding the scope of investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 415440.Google Scholar
Braidi, S. (2002). Reexamining the role of recasts in native-speaker/non-native speaker interactions. Language Learning, 52, 142.Google Scholar
Carroll, S.E. (2001). Input and evidence: The raw material of second language acquisition. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Chandler, J. (2003). The efficacy of various kinds of error feedback for improvement in the accuracy and fluency of L2 student writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 12, 267296.Google Scholar
Choi, M.-Y. (2000). Effects of recasts on irregular past tense verb morphology in web-chat. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Hawai‘i, Honolulu.
Cumming, A. (1990). Metalinguistic and ideational thinking in second language composing. Written Communication, 7, 482511.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R.M. (1995). Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 379410.Google Scholar
Egi, T. (2004). Recasts, perceptions and L2 development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgetown University, Washington, DC.
Ellis, N.C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 91126.Google Scholar
Ellis, N.C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305352.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (1994). A theory of instructed second language acquisition. In N. C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 79114). San Diego: Academic Press.
Ellis, R. (2001). Introduction: Investigating form-focused instruction. Language Learning, 51(Suppl. 1), 146.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K.A. & Simon, H.A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (Rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ferris, D. (1999). The case for grammar correction in L2 writing classes: A response to Truscott (1996). Journal of Second Language Writing, 8, 111.Google Scholar
Ferris, D. (2004). The “grammar correction” debate in L2 writing: Where are we, and where do we go from here? (and what do we do in the meantime…?). Journal of Second Language Writing, 13, 4962.Google Scholar
Fuchs, M., Fletcher, M., & Birt, D. (1986). Around the world: Pictures for practice. Book 2. London: Longman.
Gass, S.M. (1997). Input, interaction and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Iwashita, N. (2003). Positive and negative input in task-based interaction: Differential effects on L2 development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 136.Google Scholar
Jourdenais, R. (2001). Cognition, instruction and protocol analysis. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 354375). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kroll, B. (1990). What does time buy? ESL student performance on home versus class compositions. In B. Kroll (Ed.), Second language writing: Research insights for the classroom (pp. 140154). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Laufer, B. & Hulstijn, J. (2001). Incidental vocabulary acquisition in a second language: The construct of task-induced involvement. Applied Linguistics, 22, 126.Google Scholar
Leeman, J. (2003). Recasts and second language development: Beyond negative evidence. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 3763.Google Scholar
Leow, R.P. (1997). Attention, awareness, and foreign language behavior. Language Learning, 47, 467506.Google Scholar
Leow, R.P. (1999). The role of attention in second/foreign language classroom research: Methodological issues. In J. Gutierrez-Rexach & F. Martinez-Gil (Eds.), Advances in Hispanic Linguistics (pp. 6071). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Leow, R.P. (2001). Do learners notice enhanced forms while interacting with the L2? An online and offline study of the role of written input enhancement in L2 reading. Hispania, 84, 496509.Google Scholar
Leow, R.P. & Morgan-Short, K. (2004). To think aloud or not to think aloud: The issue of reactivity in SLA research methodology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 3557.Google Scholar
Loewen, S. & Philp, J. (2006). Recasts in the adult L2 classroom: Characteristics, explicitness and effectiveness. Modern Language Journal, 90, 536556.Google Scholar
Long, M.H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of language acquisition: Vol. 2. Second language acquisition (pp. 413468). San Diego: Academic Press.
Long, M.H. (2007). Problems in SLA. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lyster, R. (1998). Recasts, repetition, and ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20, 5181.Google Scholar
Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 399432.Google Scholar
Lyster, R. & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 3666.Google Scholar
Mackey, A. (1999). Input, interaction and second language development: An empirical study of question formation in ESL. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 557587.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., Gass, S.M., & McDonough, K. (2000). How do learners perceive interactional feedback? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 471497.Google Scholar
Mackey, A. & Philp, J. (1998). Conversational interaction and second language development: Recasts, responses, and red herrings? Modern Language Journal, 82, 338356.Google Scholar
Makino, T. (1993). Learner self-correction in EFL written compositions. ELT Journal, 47, 337341.Google Scholar
Nicholas, H., Lightbown, P.M., & Spada, N. (2001). Recasts as feedback to language learners. Language Learning, 51, 719758.Google Scholar
Norris, J.M. & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417528.Google Scholar
Norris, J.M. & Ortega, L. (2003). Defining and measuring L2 acquisition. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 717761). Oxford: Blackwell.
Philp, J. (2003). Constraints on “noticing the gap”: Nonnative speakers' noticing of recasts in NS-NNS interaction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 99126.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. & Johnston, M. (1987). Factors influencing the development of language proficiency. In D. Nunan (Ed.), Applying second language acquisition research (pp. 45141). Adelaide, Australia: National Curriculum Resource Centre, AMEP.
Polio, C. (1997). Measures of linguistic accuracy in second language writing research. Language Learning, 47, 101143.Google Scholar
Polio, C., Fleck, C., & Leder, N. (1998). “If only I had more time”: ESL learners' changes in linguistic accuracy on essay revisions. Journal of Second Language Writing, 7, 4368.Google Scholar
Polio, C. & Gass, S.M. (1998). The role of interaction in native speaker comprehension of nonnative speaker speech. Modern Language Journal, 82, 308319.Google Scholar
Qi, D.S. & Lapkin, S. (2001). Exploring the role of noticing in a three-stage second language writing task. Journal of Second Language Writing, 10, 277303.Google Scholar
Robb, T., Ross, S., & Shortreed, I. (1986). Salience of feedback on error and its effect on EFL writing quality. TESOL Quarterly, 20, 8395.Google Scholar
Roberts, M. (1995). Awareness and the efficacy of error correction. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (pp. 163182). Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press.
Robinson, P. (1995). Attention, memory, and the “noticing” hypothesis. Language Learning, 45, 283331.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2001). Individual differences, cognitive abilities, aptitude complexes and learning conditions in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 17, 368392.Google Scholar
Rosa, E.M. & Leow, R.P. (2004). Awareness, different learning conditions, and second language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 269292.Google Scholar
Rosa, E.M. & O'Neill, M. (1999). Explicitness, intake, and the issue of awareness: Another piece of the puzzle. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 511556.Google Scholar
Sachs, R. & Suh, B.-R., (in press). Textually enhanced recasts, learner awareness, and L2 outcomes in synchronous computer-mediated interaction. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A series of empirical studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Saxton, M. (1997). The contrast theory of negative input. Journal of Child Language, 24, 139161.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R.W. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1995). Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning. Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press.
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 332). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schmidt, R. & Frota, S. (1986). Developing basic conversational ability in a second language: A case study of an adult learner of Portuguese. In R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in second language acquisition (pp. 237326). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Sheen, Y.H. (2004). Corrective feedback and learner uptake in communicative classrooms across instructional settings. Language Teaching Research, 8, 263300.Google Scholar
Stratman, J.F. & Hamp-Lyons, L. (1994). Reactivity in concurrent think-aloud protocols: Issues for research. In P. Smagorinsky (Ed.), Speaking about writing: Reflections on research methodology (pp. 89112). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In S. M. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input and second language acquisition (pp. 235256). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. In G. Cook & B. Seidlhofer (Eds.), Principles and practice in applied linguistics (pp. 125144). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Swain, M. (1998). Focus on form through conscious reflection. In C. J. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 6481). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Swain, M. & Lapkin, S. (1995). Problems in output and the cognitive processes they generate: A step towards second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 16, 371391.Google Scholar
Thornbury, S. (1997). Reformulation and reconstruction: Tasks that promote “noticing.” ELT Journal, 51, 326335.Google Scholar
Tomlin, R.S. & Villa, V. (1994). Attention in cognitive science and second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 183203.Google Scholar
Truscott, J. (1996). The case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Language Learning, 46, 327369.Google Scholar
Truscott, J. (2004). Evidence and conjecture on the effects of correction: A response to Chandler. Journal of Second Language Writing, 13, 337343.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1990). Attending to form and content in the input. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 287301.Google Scholar
Wong, W. (2001). Modality and attention to meaning and form in the input. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 345368.Google Scholar