Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T17:46:49.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Beth E. Snitz*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A.
Frederick W. Unverzagt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, U.S.A.
Chung-Chou H. Chang
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A. Department of Biostatistics, University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A.
Joni Vander Bilt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A.
Sujuan Gao
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Division of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, U.S.A.
Judith Saxton
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A.
Kathleen S. Hall
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, U.S.A.
Mary Ganguli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A. Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health, Pittsburgh, PA, U.S.A.
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Beth E. Snitz, PhD, Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, 3471 Fifth Avenue, Suite 802, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, U.S.A. Phone: +1 412–692-4820; Fax: +1 412–692-4031. Email: snitzbe@upmc.edu.

Abstract

Background: Neuropsychological tests, including tests of language ability, are frequently used to differentiate normal from pathological cognitive aging. However, language can be particularly difficult to assess in a standardized manner in cross-cultural studies and in patients from different educational and cultural backgrounds. This study examined the effects of age, gender, education and race on performance of two language tests: the animal fluency task (AFT) and the Indiana University Token Test (IUTT). We report population-based normative data on these tests from two combined ethnically divergent, cognitively normal, representative population samples of older adults.

Methods: Participants aged ≥65 years from the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) and from the Indianapolis Study of Health and Aging (ISHA) were selected based on (1) a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) score of 0; (2) non-missing baseline language test data; and (3) race self-reported as African-American or white. The combined sample (n = 1885) was 28.1% African-American. Multivariate ordinal logistic regression was used to model the effects of demographic characteristics on test scores.

Results: On both language tests, better performance was significantly associated with higher education, younger age, and white race. On the IUTT, better performance was also associated with female gender. We found no significant interactions between age and sex, and between race and education.

Conclusions: Age and education are more potent variables than are race and gender influencing performance on these language tests. Demographically stratified normative tables for these measures can be used to guide test interpretation and aid clinical diagnosis of impaired cognition.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acevedo, A. et al. (2000). Category fluency test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 760769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Benton, A. L. and Hamsher, K. D. (1989). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: AJA Associates.Google Scholar
Bolla, K. I., Lindgren, K. N., Bonaccorsy, C. and Bleecker, M. L. (1990). Predictors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 623628.3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrd, D. A., Sanchez, D. and Manly, J. J. (2005). Neuropsychological test performance among Caribbean-born and U.S.-born African American elderly: the role of age, education and reading level. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 10561069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canadian Study of Health and Aging Working Group (1994). Canadian study of health and aging: study methods and prevalence of dementia. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 150, 899913.Google Scholar
Day, J. C. (1996). Current Population Reports. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census.Google Scholar
de Renzi, E. and Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex, 14, 4149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganguli, M., Snitz, B., Vander Bilt, J. and Chang, C. C. -H. (2009). How much do depressive symptoms affect cognition at the population level? The Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. Epublished ahead of print, doi:10.1002/gps.2257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasquoine, P. G. (1999). Variables moderating cultural and ethnic differences in neuropsychological assessment: the case of Hispanic Americans. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 376383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendrie, H. C. et al. (1995). Prevalence of Alzheimer's disease and dementia in two communities: Nigerian Africans and African Americans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 14851492.Google ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A. and Martin, R. L. (1982). A new scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 566572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. G. and Petersen, R. C. (1996). Neuropsychological tests’ norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 262278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Selfridge, M., Zalewski, C. and Aboudarham, J. (1998). The relationship between ethnicity and word fluency. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 319325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. N. and Ayers, C. R. (2006). Psychometric properties and factor structure of an expanded CERAD neuropsychological battery in an elderly VA sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 359365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamar, M., Zonderman, A. B. and Resnick, S. (2002). Contribution of specific cognitive processes to executive functioning in an aging population. Neuropsychology, 16, 156162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, J. and Fischer, J. S. (1994). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Manly, J. J. et al. (1998a). Cognitive test performance among nondemented elderly African Americans and whites. Neurology, 50, 12381245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J. J. et al. and HIV-Neurobehavioral Research Centre Group (1998b). The effect of African-American acculturation on neuropsychological test performance in normal and HIV-positive individuals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 291302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J. J., Byrd, D. A., Touradji, P. and Stern, Y. (2004). Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Applied Neuropsychology, 11, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D., Marshall, S. C., Weldon, M., Haan, M. and Reed, B. R. (1996). Age and education correction of Mini-mental State Examination for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology, 46, 700706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, W. G. (1980). Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 135146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarno, M. T., Buonaguro, A. and Levita, E. (1985). Gender and recovery from aphasia after stroke. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 605609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D. T. et al. (1998). The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 265278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swihart, A. A., Panisett, M., Becker, J.T., Beyer, J.R. and Boller, F. (1989). The Token Test: validity and diagnostic power in Alzheimer's disease. Developmental Neuropsychology, 5, 6978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 370378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unverzagt, F. W. et al. (1996). Effects of age, education, and gender on CERAD neuropsychological test performance in an African American sample. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 180190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unverzagt, F. W. et al. (1999). Clinical utility of CERAD neuropsychological battery in elderly Jamaicans. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 255259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unverzagt, F. W. et al. (2007). Mild cognitive dysfunction: an epidemiological perspective with an emphasis on African Americans. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 20, 215226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed