Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T12:08:48.627Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Activation of syllable units during visual recognition of French words in Grade 2*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2008

FABIENNE CHETAIL*
Affiliation:
Université Bordeaux 2, Bordeaux
STEPHANIE MATHEY
Affiliation:
Université Bordeaux 2, Bordeaux
*
Address for correspondence: Fabienne Chetail, Département de Psychologie, Université Bordeaux 2, 3ter Place de la Victoire, 33076 Bordeaux cedex, France. Tel:+33.5.57.57.18.00. Fax: +33.5.57.57.19.77. e-mail: fabienne.chetail@u-bordeaux2.fr

Abstract

The aim of the study was to investigate the syllable activation hypothesis in French beginning readers. Second graders performed a lexical decision task in which bisyllabic words were presented in two colours that either matched the syllable boundaries or not. The data showed that the children were sensitive to syllable match and to syllable complexity. In addition, good readers were slowed down while poor readers were speeded up by syllable match. These findings suggest that syllables are functional units of lexical access in children and that syllable activation is influenced by reading level.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by an MESR grant to the first author. We are grateful to the students, teachers and head teachers at the St. Seurin, Notre Dame and Sévigné schools for their participation. We thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Alvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition? Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 427–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ans, B., Carbonnel, S. & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist Multiple-Trace Memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review 105, 678723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, H., Vergara, M. & Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology 15, 545–48.Google ScholarPubMed
Bastien-Toniazzo, M., Magnan, A. & Bouchafa, H. (1999). Nature des représentations du langage écrit aux débuts de l'apprentissage de la lecture: un modèle interprétatif. Journal International de Psychologie 34, 4358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, J. R., Perfetti, C. A. & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Quick, automatic, and general activation of orthographic and phonological representations in young readers. Developmental Psychology 35, 319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M., Alvarez, C. J. & de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language 32, 766–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2002). Masked priming effects with syllabic neighbors in a lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28, 1228–42.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Vergara, M. & Barber, H. (2005). Early ERP effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 1803–817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C. & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology 39, 913–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., Davis, C. & Letcher, T. (1999). Neighbourhood effects on masked form priming in developing readers. Language and Cognitive Processes 14, 201224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colé, P., Magnan, A. & Grainger, J. (1999). Syllable-sized units in visual word recognition: Evidence from skilled and beginning readers of French. Applied Psycholinguistics 20, 507532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colé, P. & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (1999). Traitement syllabique au cours de la reconnaissance de mots écrits chez des enfants dyslexiques, lecteurs en retard et normo-lecteurs de 11 ans. Revue de Neuropsychologie 9, 323–60.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Carreiras, M., Tamm, S. & Jacobs, A. M. (in press). Syllables and bigrams: Orthographic redundancy and syllabic units affect visual word recognition at different processing levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Phonology as the source of syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from French. Memory & Cognition 35, 974–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denton, C. A., Hasbrouck, J. E., Weaver, L. R. & Riccio, C. A. (2000). What do we know about phonological awareness in Spanish? Reading Psychology 21, 335–52.Google Scholar
Doignon, N. & Zagar, D. (2006). Les enfants en cours d'apprentissage de la lecture perçoivent-ils la syllabe à l'écrit? Revue Canadienne de Psychologie 60, 258–74.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 35, 116–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geudens, A. & Sandra, D. (1999). Onsets and rimes in a phonologically transparent orthography: Differences between good and poor beginning readers of Dutch. Brain and Language 68, 284–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutzler, F., Bergmann, J., Conrad, M., Kronbichler, M., Stenneken, P. & Jacobs, A. M. (2004). Inhibitory effects of first syllable-frequency in lexical decision: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters 372, 179–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiménez Gonzalez, J. E. & HernandezVallé, I. Vallé, I. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities 33, 4460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lefavrais, P. (1965). Description, définition et mesure de la dyslexie. Utilisation du test ‘L'Alouette’. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée 15, 3344.Google Scholar
Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Colé, P. (2004). MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 36, 156–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W. & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 18, 201212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathey, S. & Zagar, D. (2002). Lexical similarity in visual word recognition: The effect of syllabic neighbourhood in French. Current Psychology Letters 8, 107121.Google Scholar
Mathey, S., Zagar, D., Doignon, N. & Seigneuric, A. (2006). The nature of syllabic neighbourhood in French. Acta Psychologica 123, 372–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88, 375407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., Dommergues, J., Frauenfelder, U. & Segui, J. (1981). The syllable's role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 298305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morais, J., Content, A., Cary, L., Mehler, J. & Segui, J. (1989). Syllabic segmentation and literacy. Language and Cognitive Processes 4, 5767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55, 151218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Siegel, L. S. (1997). A longitudinal study of the effects of syllabic structure on the development of reading and spelling skills in French. Applied Psycholinguistics 18, 485505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar