Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T11:45:08.241Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between lexicon and grammar at the end of the second year in Finnish children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2008

SUVI STOLT*
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki and Turku University Central Hospital, Finland
LEENA HAATAJA
Affiliation:
Turku University Central Hospital, Finland
HELENA LAPINLEIMU
Affiliation:
Turku University Central Hospital, Finland
LIISA LEHTONEN
Affiliation:
Turku University Central Hospital, Finland
*
Address for correspondence: Suvi Stolt, University of Turku, Faculty of Social Sciences/Logopedics, Itäinen Pitkäkatu 3a, 20014 Turku, Finland. Email: suvi.stolt@utu.fi

Abstract

The emergence of grammar in relation to lexical growth was analyzed in a sample of Finnish children (N=181) at 2 ; 0. The Finnish version of the Communicative Development Inventory was used to gather information on both language domains. The onset of grammar occurred in close association with vocabulary growth. The acquisition of the nominal and verbal inflections of Finnish differed when analyzed in relation to the lexicon in which they are used: the strongest growth in the acquisition of case form types occurred when the nominal lexicon size was roughly between 50 and 250 words, whereas verb inflectional types were acquired actively from the beginning of the verb lexicon acquisition. The findings extend the previous findings of the close association between lexicon and grammar (e.g. Bates & Goodman, 1999). The results suggest that different grammatical structures display different degrees and types of lexical dependency.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This study was supported by grants from the Emil Aaltonen Foundation, the Friends of Paediatric Clinics in University Hospitals in Finland Fund, the Neonatal Research in Southwestern Finland Fund, The Finnish Cultural Foundation, Langnet, the Finnish postgraduate school in language studies, and by the Academy of Finland. We thank Petriina Munck for recruiting the sample of 146 children, and Riikka Korja, Annika Lind, Petriina Munck and Anu Uschakoff, psychologists, for assessing the cognitive skills of the children at 2 ; 0. We thank Jaakko Matomäki for help in data analyses. We are grateful for all the families for participating in this study. This study is part of the PIPARI study. The PIPARI study group comprises Mikael Ekblad, Satu Ekblad, Eeva Ekholm, Leena Haataja, Pentti Kero, Jarkko Kirjavainen, Riikka Korja, Harry Kujari, Helena Lapinleimu, Liisa Lehtonen, Annika Lind, Hanna Manninen, Jaakko Matomäki, Jonna Maunu, Petriina Munck, Pekka Niemi, Pertti Palo, Riitta Parkkola, Anniina Peltola, Jorma Piha, Liisi Rautava, Päivi Rautava, Milla Reiman, Hellevi Rikalainen, Katriina Saarinen, Elina Savonlahti, Matti Sillanpää, Suvi Stolt, Päivi Tuomikoski-Koiranen and Tuula Äärimaa.

References

REFERENCES

Anisfeld, M., Rosenberg, E., Hoberman, M. & Gasparini, D. (1998). Lexical acceleration coincides with the onset of combinatorial speech. First Language 18, 165–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Bretherton, I. & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Dale, P. & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds), The handbook of child language, 96151. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processing 12, 507584.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Goodman, J. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The emergence of language, 2980. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Reilly, J. & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21, 85123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (1993). The Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bittner, D., Dressler, W. & Kilani-Schoch, M. (2003). Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition. A cross-linguistic perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M., Leach, D. & Haynes, M. (2004). Vocabulary competence in first- and secondborn siblings of the same chronological age. Journal of Child Language 31, 855–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M., Putnick, D. & De Houwer, A. (2006). Child vocabularly across the second year: Stability and continuity for reporter comparisons and a cumulative score. First Language 26, 299316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, C., Casadio, P. & Bates, E. (1999). A comparison of the transition from first words to grammar in English and Italian. Journal of Child Language 26, 69111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. (2003). First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dasinger, L. (1997). Issues in the acquisition of Estonian, Finnish, and Hungarian: A crosslinguistic comparison. In Dan Slobin, (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 4, 87182. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dionne, G., Dale, P., Boivin, M. & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development 74, 394412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, J. & Marchman, V. (2007). Grammar and the lexicon: Developmental ordering in language acquisition. Child Development 78, 190212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D. & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, (5, Serial No. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. (eds), Language acquisition and conceptual development, 215–56. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meaning. Language Acquisition 1, 355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1999). The emergence of the semantics of argument structure constructions. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The emergence of language, 197212. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Häkkinen, K. (2001). Kielitieteen perusteet. [Basics of linguistics] Helsinki: Suomalaisen kirjallisuuden seura. [Finnish literature association].Google Scholar
Laalo, K. (2002). Acquisition of case in Finnish: A preliminary overview. In Voeikova, M. & Dressler, W. (eds), Pre- and protomorphology. Early phases of morphological Development in nouns and verbs, Vol. 29, 83103. Lincom Studies in Theoretical Linguistics. Munchen: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Laalo, K. (2003). Early verb development in Finnish: A preliminary approach to miniparadigms. In Bittner, D., Dressler, W. & Kilani-Schoch, M. (eds), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition, 323–50. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lyytinen, P. (1999). Varhaisen kommunikaation ja kielen kehityksen arviointimenetelmä. [The method to evalute early communication and language]. Jyväskylä: Niilo Mäki Säätiö. [Niilo Mäki Foundation].Google Scholar
Lyytinen, P. & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Growth and predictive relations of vocabulary and inflectional morphology in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics 25, 397411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maital, S., Dromi, E., Sagi, A. & Bornstein, M. (2000). The Hebrew Communicative Development Inventory: Language specific properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. Journal of Child Language 27, 4367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 21, 339–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V., Plunkett, K. & Goodman, J. (1997). Overregularization in English plural and past tense inflectional morphology: A response to Marcus (1995). Journal of Child Language 24, 767–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. (1996). Why do children say ‘breaked’? Current Directions in Psychological Science 5, 8185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyle, M., Weismer, S., Evans, J. & Lindstrom, M. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 50, 508528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. (1995). Strategies in the acquisition of syntax. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (eds), The handbook of child language, 462–82. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science 253, 530–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H. & Lehtonen, L. (2008). Early lexical development of Finnish children – a longitudinal study. First Language 28, 259–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolt, S., Klippi, A., Launonen, K., Munck, P., Lehtonen, L., Lapinleimu, H., Haataja, L. & the PIPARI study group (2007). Size and composition of the lexicon in prematurely born very-low-birth-weight and full-term Finnish children at two years of age. Journal of Child Language 34, 283310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szagun, G., Steinbrink, C., Franik, M. & Stumper, B. (2006). Development of vocabulary and grammar in young German-speaking children assessed with a German language development inventory. First Language 26, 259–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, E., Weismer, S. & Evans, J. (2002). Continuity in lexical and morphological development in Icelandic and English-speaking 2-year-olds. First Language 22, 228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toivainen, J. (1980). Inflectional affixes used by Finnish-speaking children aged 1–3 years. Helsinki: Suomalaisen kirjallisuuden seura. [Finnish literature association].Google Scholar
Toivainen, J. (1997). The acquisition of Finnish. In Slobin, Dan (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 4, 87182. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar