Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T08:06:34.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infant response in the still-face paradigm at 6 months predicts avoidant and secure attachment at 12 months

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2009

Jeffrey F. Cohn*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Susan B. Campbell*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Shelley Ross*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
*
Address reprint requests to: J. F. Cohn, S. B. Campbell, or S. Ross, University of Pittsburgh, Clinical Psychology Program, 4015 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15260.
Address reprint requests to: J. F. Cohn, S. B. Campbell, or S. Ross, University of Pittsburgh, Clinical Psychology Program, 4015 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15260.
Address reprint requests to: J. F. Cohn, S. B. Campbell, or S. Ross, University of Pittsburgh, Clinical Psychology Program, 4015 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15260.

Abstract

We tested the hypothesis that attachment security at 12 months can be predicted from infant response to the mother's still-face interaction during the first half-year. Subjects were 66 primiparous mother-infant pairs drawn from the working- and middle-socioeconomic strata (SES). Half of the mothers had experienced a postpartum depression. Infants were observed longitudinally in the still-face paradigm at 2, 4, and 6 months and in the Strange Situation at 12 months. Positive expressions in response to the still-face at 6 months predicted secure attachment at 12 months. Absence of positive expressions predicted avoidant attachment. In infants of postpartum depressed mothers, hours of nonmaternal care positively correlated with secure attachment. Variables associated with infant proneness to distress and sociability were unrelated to attachment security. These results suggest that attachment formation is underway by 6 months of age and that indicators of 12-month attachment security can be detected as early as 6 months. Similar mechanisms of affect regulation and coping may underlie infant response in the still-face and Strange Situation procedures.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. B., & Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Measurement Of Infant Difficultness. Child Development, 53, 794803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (1988) “Effects” of infant daycare reconsidered. Early Education Quarterly, 3, 235272.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Fish, M., & Isabella, R. (1991). Continuity and discontinuity in infant negative and positive emotionality: Family antecedents and attachment consequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 421431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Rovine, M., & Taylor, D. G. (1984). The Pennsylvania Infant and Family Development Project: HI. The origins of individual differences in mother-infant attachment: Maternal and infant contributions. Child Development, 55, 718728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. (1987). New perspectives on attachment relations: Security, communication, and internal working models. In Osofsky, J. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 10611100). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brownell, C. A. (1986). Cognitive correlates of early social development. In Whitehurst, G. J. (Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 3, pp. 151). Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. B., & Cohn, J. F. (1991). Prevalence and correlates of postpartum depression in first-time mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 594599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., Flanagan, C., Popper, S., & Meyers, T. (in press). The course and correlates of depression during the transition to parenthood. Development and Psychopathology, 4(1).Google Scholar
Campos, J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In Haith, M. M. & Campos, J. J. (Eds.), Mussen, P. H. (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. II): Infancy and developmental psychobiology (pp. 783916). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1990). An historical perspective on the discipline of developmental psychopathology. In Rolf, J., Masten, A., Cicchetti, D., Neuchterlein, K., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 228). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1986). Early precursors of later depression: An organizational perspective. In Lipsitt, L. & Rovee-Collier, C. (Eds.), Advances in infancy (Vol. 4, pp. 88137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Clarke-Stewart, A. (1989). Infant day care: Maligned or malignant. American Psychologist, 44, 266273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., & Campbell, S. B. (in press). Influence of maternal depression on infant affect regulation. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 4. A developmental approach to affective disorders. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Campbell, S. B., & Hopkins, J. (1990). Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed mother-infant pairs at 2 months. Developmental Psychology, 26, 1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Tronick, E. Z., Lyons-Ruth, K., & Connell, D. (1986). Face-to-face interactions, spontaneous and structured, of mothers with depressive symptoms. In Field, T. & Tronick, E. Z. (Eds.), Maternal depression and child disturbance (New Directions for Child Development, No. 34, pp. 3146). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants' reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54, 185193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1987). Mother-infant interaction: The sequence of dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9 months. Developmental Psychology, 23, 6877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). Mother-infant interaction: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner's behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24, 386392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Specificity of infants' response to mothers' affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 242248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, E. M., & Cicchetti, D. (1990). Toward a transactional model of relations between attachment and depression. In Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 339372). Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Egeland, B., & Farber, E. A. (1984). Infant-mother attachment: Factors related to its development and change over time. Child Development, 55, 753771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 527532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Sandberg, D., Garcia, R., Vega-Lahr, N., Goldstein, S., & Guy, L. (1985). Pregnancy problems, postpartum depression, and early mother-infant interactions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 11521156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., & Alansky, J. A. (1987). Maternal and infant temperamental predictors of attachment: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 805816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of mother-infant attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62, 373384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (1987). Psychological research on the human infant: An evaluative summary. New York: William T. Grant Foundation.Google Scholar
Kiser, L. J., Bates, J. E., Maslin, C. A., & Bayles, K. (1986). Mother-infant play at 6 months as a predictor of attachment security at 13 months. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 6875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., Charnov, E. L., & Estes, D. (1984). Security of infantile attachment as assessed in the “strange situation”: Its study and biological interpretation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(1), 127147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lester, B., Hoffman, J., & Brazelton, T. B. (1985). The rhythmic structure of mother-infant interaction in term and preterm infants. Child Development, 56, 1527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M., & Feiring, C. (1989). Infant, mother, and mother-infant interaction behavior and subsequent attachment. Child Development, 60, 831837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D. B., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 223232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Zoll, D., Connell, D. B., & Grunebaum, H. U. (1986). The depressed mother and her one-year-old infant: Environment, interaction, attachment and infant development. In Field, T. & Tronick, E. Z. (Eds.), Maternal depression and child disturbance (New Directions for Child Development, No. 34, pp. 6182). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C., Tesman, J. R., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54(1–2, Serial No. 219).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J. A. (1981). A longitudinal study of the consequences of early mother-infant interaction: A microanalytic approach. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(3, Serial No. 190).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, M.Kuczynski, L., & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal families and in families with parental depression. Child Development, 56, 884893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 3, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schafer, H. R. (1984). The child's entry into a social world. London: Academic.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. S., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1978). Research Diagnostic Criteria: Rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 773782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1989). The representation of relational patterns: Developmental perspectives. In Sameroff, A. J. & Emde, R. (Eds.), Relationship disturbances in early childhood (pp. 5269). New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Teti, D. M., & Gelfand, D. M. (1990). Maternal competence and characteristics of mother, infant, and environment: The mediational role of maternal self-efficacy. Submitted.Google Scholar
Tronick, E., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1980). Monadic phases: A structural descriptive analysis of face-to-face interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 324.Google Scholar
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, J., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E., Ricks, M., & Cohn, J. F. (1982). Maternal and infant affective exchange: Patterns of adaptation. In Field, T. & Fogel, A. (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction (pp. 83100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar