Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T16:39:39.430Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Role of working memory in children's understanding spoken narrative: A preliminary investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2009

JAMES W. MONTGOMERY*
Affiliation:
Ohio University
ANZHELA POLUNENKO
Affiliation:
Hillsborough Public School System, North Carolina
SALLY A. MARINELLIE
Affiliation:
Ohio University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE James W. Montgomery, School of Hearing, Speech, and Language Sciences, W231, Grover Center, College of Health & Human Services, Ohio University, Athens, OH 45701. E-mail: montgoj1@ohio.edu

Abstract

The role of phonological short-term memory (PSTM), attentional resource capacity/allocation, and processing speed on children's spoken narrative comprehension was investigated. Sixty-seven children (6–11 years) completed a digit span task (PSTM), concurrent verbal processing and storage (CPS) task (resource capacity/allocation), auditory–visual reaction time (AVRT) task (processing speed), and the Test of Narrative Language. Correlation and regression analyses examined the association between the memory variables and comprehension. The main findings were (a) CPS and AVRT correlated with comprehension and (b) after accounting for age, CPS accounted for a significant 7.9% of unique variance and AVRT accounted for another significant 5.2%. The results indicate that resource capacity/allocation and processing speed are important to children's ability to understand spoken narrative.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alloway, T., Gathercole, S., & Pickering, S. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 16981716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Speech–Language–Hearing Association. (1990). Guidelines for screening hearing impairment and middle ear disorders. ASHA, 32 (Suppl. 2), 1724.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory and learning. In Broadbent, D., McGaugh, J., Kosslyn, M., Mackintosh, N., Tulving, E., & Weiskrantz, L. (Eds.), Working memory (pp. 3353). New York: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1998). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 4790). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. (2002). Prose recall and amnesia: Implications for the structure of working memory. Neuropsychologia, 40, 17371743.Google Scholar
Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2001). Developmental increase in working memory span: Resource sharing or temporal decay? Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayliss, M., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A., Gunn, D., & Leigh, E. (2005). Mapping the developmental constraints on working memory span performance. Developmental Psychology, 41, 579597.Google Scholar
Bayliss, M., Jarrold, C., Gunn, D., & Baddeley, A. (2003). The complexities of complex span: Explaining Individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 7192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic development study. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. (2003). Test for reception of grammar—2. London: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Booth, J., MacWhinney, B., & Harasaki, Y. (2000). Developmental differences in visual and auditory processing of complex sentences. Child Development, 71, 9811003.Google Scholar
Bornens, M. (1990). Problems brought about by “reading” a sequence of pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 189226.Google ScholarPubMed
Botvin, G., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1977). The development of complexity in children's fantasy narratives. Developmental Psychology, 13, 377388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G., & Morrow, D. (1990). Mental models in narrative comprehension. Science, 247, 4448.Google Scholar
Bowers, J., Vigliocco, G., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Vinson, D. (1999). Distinguishing language from thought: Experimental evidence that syntax is lexically rather than conceptually represented. Psychological Science, 10, 310315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R., & Johnsen, S. (1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Cain, K, Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A., & Engle, R. (1996). Individual differences in working memory capacity: More evidence for a general capacity theory. Memory, 4, 577590.Google Scholar
Conway, A., Kane, M., Bunting, M., Hambrick, D., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (1997). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Elliott, E., Saults, S., Morey, C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., et al. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 42100.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422433.Google Scholar
Dick, F., Wulfeck, B., Krupa-Kwiatkowski, M., & Bates, L. (2004). The development of complex sentence interpretation in typically developing children compared with children with specific language impairment or early unilateral focal lesions. Developmental Science, 7, 360377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickenson, D., & Snow, E. (1987). Interrelationships among prereading and oral language skills in kindergartners from two social classes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2, 126.Google Scholar
Dickenson, D., & Tabors, P. (1991). Early literacy: Linkages between home, school, and literacy achievement at age five. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 3046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12491260.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., & Thordardottir, E. (2002). Cognition and language. In Accardo, P., Rogers, B., & Capute, A. (Eds.), Disorders of language development (pp. 2137). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.Google Scholar
Ferguson, A. N., & Bowey, J. A. (2005). Global processing speed as a mediator of developmental changes in children's auditory memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 89112.Google Scholar
Fry, A., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237241.Google Scholar
Fry, A., & Hale, S. (2000). Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Psychological Science, 7, 237241.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Cognitive Science, 3, 410419.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177190.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.Google Scholar
Gaulin, C., & Campbell, T. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 5564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavens, N., & Barrouillet, P. (2004). Delays of retention, processing efficiency, and attentional resources in working memory span development. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 644657.Google Scholar
Genereux, R., & McKeough, A. (2007). Developing narrative interpretation: Structural and content analyses. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 849872.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. (1997). Two decades of structure building. Discourse Processes, 23, 265304.Google Scholar
Gerrig, R., & O'Brien, E. (2005). The scope of memory-based processing. Discourse Processes, 39, 225242.Google Scholar
Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A. (2004). Test of Narrative Language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Graesser, A., Singer, M, & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371395.Google Scholar
Hale, S. (1990). A global developmental trend in cognitive processing speed in children. Child Development, 61, 653663.Google Scholar
Holsgrove, J., & Garton, A. (2006). Phonological and syntactic processing and the role of working memory in reading comprehension among secondary school students. Australian Journal of Psychology, 58, 111118.Google Scholar
Hitch, G., Towse, J., & Hutton, U. (2001). What limits children's working memory span? Theoretical accounts and applications for scholastic development. Journal of Developmental Psychology: General, 130, 184198.Google Scholar
Hudson, J., & Shapiro, L. (1991). From knowing to telling: The development of children's scripts, stories, and personal event narratives. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in school children and adults. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 35(Serial No. 134, No. 1).Google Scholar
Irwin-Chase, H., & Burns, B. (2000). Developmental changes in children's abilities to share and allocate attention in a dual task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 6185.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. (1993). Why is processing speed correlated with psychometric g? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 5356.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Just, M., & Carpenter, P. (1992). A capacity theory comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122149.Google Scholar
Kail, R. (1991). Processing time declines exponentially during childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 259266.Google Scholar
Kail, R. (1992). Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. Developmental Psychology, 28, 899904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (2000). Speed of information processing: Developmental change and links to intelligence. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 5161.Google Scholar
Kail, R., & Miller, C. (2006). Developmental change in processing speed: Domain specificity and stability during childhood and adolescence. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 119137.Google Scholar
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199225.Google Scholar
Kane, M., Bleckley, M., Conway, A., & Engle, R. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169183.Google Scholar
Leather, C., & Henry, L. (1994). Working memory span and phonological awareness tasks as predictors of early reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 88111.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C., Francis, D., Tomblin, J., & Kail, R. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 408428.Google Scholar
Lewis, R., Vasishth, S., & van Dyke, J. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 447454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Love, T. (2007). The processing non-canonically ordered constituents in long distance dependencies by preschool children: A real-time investigation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 191206.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In Kuczaj, S. (Ed.), Language acquisition: Vol. 1. Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mainela-Arnold, E., & Evans, J. (2005). Beyond capacity limitations: Determinants of word recall performance on verbal working memory tasks by children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 897909.Google Scholar
McElree, B., Foraker, S., & Dyer, L. (2003). Memory structures that subserve sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 6791.Google Scholar
McKeough, A. (1992). A neo-structural analysis of children's narrative and its development. In Case, R. (Ed.), The mind's staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children's thought and knowledge (pp. 171188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McKeough, A. (2000). Building on the oral tradition: How story composition and comprehension develop. In Astington, J. (Ed.), Minds in the making (pp. 98114). Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99, 440466.Google Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1998). Memory-based language processing: Psycholinguistic research in the 1990s. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 2542.Google Scholar
Merritt, D., & Liles, B. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation and story retelling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 438447.Google Scholar
Moe, A., Hopkins, C., & Rush, R. (1982). Vocabulary of first-grade children. Springfield, IL: Thomas.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. (2005). Effects of input rate and age on the real-time lexical processing of children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 171188.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J., & Evans, J. (in press). Complex sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: Influence of working memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J., & Leonard, L. (2006). Effects of acoustic manipulation on the real-time inflectional processing of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12381256.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J., Magimairaj, B., & O'Malley, M. (2008). The role of working memory in typically developing children's complex sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37, 331354.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J., & Windsor, J. (2007). Examining the language performances of children with and without specific language impairment: Contributions of phonological short-term memory and processing speed. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 778797.Google Scholar
Paris, A., & Paris, S. (2003). Assessing narrative comprehension in children. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 3676.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1991). Linking children's connective use and narrative macrostructure. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 2954). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Purcell, S., & Liles, B. (1992). Cohesion repairs in the narratives of normal-language and language disordered school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 354362.Google Scholar
Roberts, L., Marinis, T., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2007). Antecedent priming at trace positions in children's sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 175188.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing. Psychological Science, 2, 179183.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A. & Zuccolott, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Scott, C. M. (1988). Spoken and written syntax. In Nippold, M. A. (Ed.), Later language development: Ages nine through nineteen (pp. 4995). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Seigneuric, A., & Ehrlich, M.-F. (2005). Contribution of working capacity to children's reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing, 18, 617656.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Singer, M., Graesser, A., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Minimal or global inference during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 421441.Google Scholar
Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Swanson, L., & Berninger, V. (1995). The role of working memory in skilled and less skilled readers' comprehension. Intelligence, 21, 83108.Google Scholar
Tomblin, B., Records, N., Buckwalter, N., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 12451260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Towse, J., Hitch, G., & Hutton, U. (1998). A reevaluation of working memory capacity in children. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 195217.Google Scholar
Towse, J., Hitch, G., & Hutton, U. (2002). On the nature of the relationship between processing activity and item retention in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 156184.Google Scholar
Tyler, A. (1992). Spoken language comprehension: An experimental approach to disordered and normal processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: The development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In van den Broek, P., Bauer, P., & Bourg, T. (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension: Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 321342). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
van den Broek, P., Lynch, J. S., Naslund, J., Ievers-Landis, C. E., & Verduin, K. (2003). The development of comprehension of main ideas in narratives: Evidence from the selection of titles. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 707718.Google Scholar
van Dijk, T., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., & Hwang, M. (1999). Children's auditory lexical decisions: A limited processing capacity account of language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 9901002.Google Scholar
Yuill, N., Oakhill, J., & Parkin, A. (1989). Working memory, comprehension ability and the resolution of text anomaly. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 351361.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. (2004). The immersed experience: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 44, pp. 3562). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R., & Radvansky, G. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162185.Google Scholar